Skip to main content
Log in

Basic Emotion Recognition According to Clinical Personality Traits

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Disturbances in the ability to recognize emotional faces have been attributed to individuals with specific personality disorders. Considering the importance of the dimensional models of psychopathology, studies involving healthy participants are becoming increasingly relevant in the domain of personality disorders. In this context, our main goal was to assess how clinical personality traits affect the ability to recognize basic emotions in a sample of subclinical participants. Photographs of faces expressing six basic emotions (happiness, sadness, fear, anger, disgust and surprise) were presented to 72 undergraduate students (42 women; M age = 23.3 yr., SD = 3.4), whose dominant personality traits (narcissistic, histrionic and compulsive) were assessed using the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-III. Data were analyzed using both a whole sample regression approach (relating personality traits with emotion recognition performance) and a group comparison approach (comparing groups of participants with dominant personality - narcissistic, histrionic and compulsive - as well as comparing groups with subclinical symptomatology for anxiety and hypomania). The main results suggested a poor recognition of sadness in narcissistic participants and a higher difficulty for anger recognition in participants with anxiety symptoms. These results are discussed within the theoretical framework suggesting that the difficulties in basic and social emotions recognition have implications in interpersonal interactions experienced in different social contexts.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abramowitz, J. S., Fabricant, L. E., Taylor, S., Deacon, B. J., McKay, D., & Storch, E. A. (2014). The relevance of analogue studies for understanding obsessions and compulsions. Clinical Psychology Review, 34(3), 206–217. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2014.01.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ali, F., & Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2010). Investigating theory of mind deficits in nonclinical psychopathy and Machiavellianism. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 169–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Manual de Diagnóstico e Estatística das Perturbações Mentais, 4ª Edição, Revisão de Texto. Lisboa: Climepsi Editores.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ames, D., & Kammrath, L. (2004). Mind-reading and metacognition: Narcissism not actual competence, predicts self-estimated ability. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28(3), 187–209. doi:10.1023/B:JONB.0000039649.20015.0e.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). Theory of mind and autism: A review. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 23, 169–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 37–46. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baskin-Sommers, A., Krusemark, E., & Ronningstam, E. (2014). Empathy in narcissistic personality disorder: From clinical and empirical perspectives. Personality Disorders, 5(3), 323–333 http://doi.org/10.1037/per0000061.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baughman, H. M., Dearing, S., Giammarco, E., & Vernon, P. A. (2012). Relationships between bullying behaviours and the dark triad: A study with adults. Personality and Individual Differences, 52, 571–575. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.11.020.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Freeman, A., & Davis, D. D. (2005). Terapia Cognitiva dos Transtornos de Personalidade. Porto Alegre: Artmed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bird, C. M., Castelli, F., Malik, O., Frith, U., & Husain, M. (2004). The impact of extensive medial frontal lobe damage on `Theory of Mind' and cognition. Brain, 127, 914–928. doi:10.1093/brain/awh108.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, S. J. (2007). Neurocognitive mechanisms of anxiety: An integrative account. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(7), 307–316. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, R. J., Mitchell, D. G., Peschardt, K. S., Colledge, E., Leonard, R. A., Shine, J. H., Murray, L. K., & Perrett, D. I. (2004). Reduced sensitivity to others’ fearful expressions in psychopathic individuals. Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 1111–1122. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2003.10.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bocharov, A. V., & Knyazev, G. G. (2011). Interaction of anger with anxiety and responses to emotional facial expressions. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(3), 398–403. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.11.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bora, E., & Berk, M. (2016). Theory of mind in major depressive disorder: A meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 191, 49–55. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2015.11.023.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bora, E., Walterfang, W., & Velakoulis, D. (2015). Theory of mind in Parkinson's disease: A meta-analysis. Behavioural Brain Research, 292(1), 515–520. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2015.07.012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brook, M., & Kosson, D. (2013). Impaired cognitive empathy in criminal psychopathy: Evidence from a laboratory measure of empathic accuracy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122(1), 156–166. doi:10.1037/a0030261.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brüne, M. (2005). ‘Theory of Mind’’ in schizophrenia: A review of the literature. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 31(1), 21–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Calder, A. J., Ewbank, M., & Passamonti, L. (2011). Personality influences the neuronal responses to viewing facial expressions of emotion. Philophical Transactions of The Royal Society B, 366(1571), 1684–1701. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campanella, S., Vanhoolandt, M. E., & Philippot, P. (2005). Emotional deficit in subjects with psychopathic tendencies as assessed by the MMPI-2: An event-related potentials study. Neuroscience Letters, 373, 26–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carter, G. L., Campbell, A. C., & Muncer, S. (2014). The dark triad: Beyond a ‘male’ mating strategy. Personality and Individual Differences, 56, 159–164. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2013.09.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cavaco, S. (2008). Trail making test: dados normativos dos 21 aos 65 anos. Psychologica, 49, 222–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corcoran, R. (2000). Theory of mind in other clinical conditions: Is selective ‘theory of mind’ deficit exclusive to autism? In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds (2nd ed., pp. 391–421). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Couture, S. S., Penn, D. L., & Roberts, D. L. (2006). The functional significance of social cognition in schizophrenia: A review. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 32, 44–63. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbl029.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawel, A., O’Kearney, R., McKone, E., & Palermo, R. (2012). Not just fear and sadness: Meta-analytic evidence of pervasive emotion recognition deficits for facial and vocal expressions in psychopathy. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(10), 2288–22304. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.08.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dolan, M., & Fullam, R. (2004). Theory of mind and mentalizing ability in antisocial personality disorders with and without psychopathy. Psychological Medicine, 34(6), 1093–1102. doi:10.1017/S0033291704002028.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dolan, M., & Fullam, R. (2006). Face affect recognition deficits in personality disordered offenders: Association with psychopathy. Psychological Medicine, 36, 1563–1569.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Domes, G., Czieschnek, D., Weidler, F., Berger, C., Fast, K., & Herpertz, S. C. (2008). Recognition of facial affect in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 22(2), 135–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edgar, C., McRorie, M., & Sneddon, I. (2012). Emotional intelligence, personality and the decoding of non-verbal expressions of emotion. Personality and Individual Differences, 52, 295–300. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.10.024.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 169–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W., & Keane, M. T. (2010). Cognitive psychology: A Student’s handbook (6nd ed.). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feist, J., Feist, G., & Robert, T. (2012). Theory of personality. New York: McGraw Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamann, S., & Canli, T. (2004). Individual differences in emotion processing. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14(2), 233–238. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A., & Gobbini, M. I. (2002). The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 223–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herpertz, S. C., & Bertsch, K. (2014). The social-cognitive basis of personality disorders. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 27(1), 73–77. doi:10.1097/YCO.0000000000000026.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jakobwitz, S., & Egan, V. (2006). The dark triad and normal personality traits. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 331–339. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.07.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kessler, H., Bayerl, P., Deighton, R. M., & Traue, H. C. (2002). Facially expressed emotion labelling (feel): Pc-gestutzter test zur emotions erkennung. Verhaltenstherapie und Verhaltensmedizin, 23, 297–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knyazev, G. G., Bocharov, A. V., Slobodskaya, H. R., & Ryabichek, T. I. (2008). Personality-linked biases in perception of emotional facial expressions. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 1093–1104. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.11.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kosson, D. S., Suchy, Y., Mayer, A. R., & Libby, J. (2002). Facial affect recognition in criminal psychopaths. Emotion, 2(4), 398–411. doi:10.1037//1528-3542.2.4.398.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krippendorff, K. (2004). Content analysis. An introduction to its methodology. London: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., & Loring, D. W. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment (4th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marissen, M. A., Deen, M. L., & Franken, I. H. (2012). Disturbed emotion recognition in patients with narcissistic personality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 198(2), 269–273. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2011.12.042.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, A. A., & Blair, R. J. (2008). Deficits in facial affect recognition among antisocial populations: A meta-analysis. Neuroscience Biobehavioural Review, 32(3), 454–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martins, A., & Reis, A. (2007). Validação de estímulos para construção de paradigmas para o estudo do reconhecimento de emoções. Provas de Aptidão e Capacidade Científica pela Universidade do Algarve.

  • Martins, A., Muresan, M., Justo, M., & Simão, C. (2008). Basic and social emotion recognition in patients with Parkinson disease. Journal of Neurological Sciences, 25, 247–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martins, A., Faísca, L., Esteves, F., Muresan, A., Justo, M., Simão, C., & Reis, A. (2011). Traumatic brain injury patients: Does frontal brain lesion influence basic emotion recognition? Psychology & Neuroscience, 4(3), 377–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mennin, D. S., Heimberg, R. G., Turk, C. L., & Fresco, D. M. (2005). Preliminary evidence for an emotion dysregulation model of generalized anxiety disorder. Research Therapy, 43, 1281–1310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Millon, T., Millon, C., Davis, R., & Grossman, S. (2009). MCMI-III: Millon clinical multiaxial inventory manual (4th ed.). Minneapolis: Pearson Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Millon, T., Grossman, S., & Millon, C. (2015). MCMI-IV: Millon clinical multiaxial inventory manual (1st ed.). Bloomington: NCS Pearson, Inc..

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, R. L. C., & Young, A. H. (2016). Theory of mind in bipolar disorder, with comparison to the impairments observed in schizophrenia. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 6, 188. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00188.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • O’Boyle, E. H., Forsyth, D. R., Banks, G. C., & McDaniel, M. A. (2012). A meta-analysis of the dark triad and work behaviour: A social exchange perspective. Journal of Applied Psychology, 97, 557–579. doi:10.1037/a0025679.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Orgeta, V. (2011). Emotion dysregulation and anxiety in late adulthood. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 25, 1019–1023.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paulhus, D., & Williams, K. (2002). The dark triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. Journal of Research in Personality, 36, 556–563. doi:10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00505-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pawluk, E. J., & Koerner, N. (2013). A preliminary investigation of impulsivity in generalized anxiety disorder. Personality and Individual Differences, 54, 732–737.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Penney, L. M., & Spector, P. E. (2002). Narcissism and counterproductive work behavior: Do bigger egos mean bigger problems? International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 10, 126–134. doi:10.1111/1468- 2389.00199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (2000). Raven manual: Section 3, standard progressive matrices, including the parallel and plus versions, 2000 edition. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reitan, R. M. (1958). Validity of the trail making test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 8, 271–276. doi:10.2466/pms.1958.8.3.271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richell, R. A., Mitchell, D. G., Newman, C., Leonard, A., Baron-Cohen, S., & Blair, R. J. (2003). Theory of mind and psychopathy: Can psychopathic individuals read the ‘language of the eyes’? Neuropsychologia, 41, 523–526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ritter, K., Dziobek, I., Preißler, S., Rüter, A., Vater, A., Fydrich, T., Lammers, C., Heekeren, H., & Roepke, S. (2011). Lack of empathy in patients with narcissistic personality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 187, 241–247. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2010.09.013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rocha, H. P., Sousa, H. C., Alchieri, J. C., Sales, E., & Alencar, J. N. (2011). Estudos de adaptação do Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-III para avaliação de aspetos psicopatológicos da personalidade no Brasil. Jornal Brasileiro de Psiquiatria, 60(1), 34–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roczniewska, M., & Bakker, A. B. (2016). Who seeks job resources, and who avoids job demands? The link between dark personality traits and job crafting. The Journal of Psychology, 150, 1026–1045. doi:10.1080/00223980.2016.1235537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sabbagh, M. A., Moulson, M. C., & Harkness, K. L. (2004). Neural correlates of mental state decoding in human adults: An event-related potential study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(3), 415–426. doi:10.1162/089892904322926755.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simão, C., Mariline, J., & Martins, A. T. (2009). Recognizing facial expressions of social emotions: Do males and females differ? Psicologia, 22(2), 71–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spain, S. M., Harms, P. D., & LeBreton, J. (2014). The dark side of personality at work. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 35, S41–S60. doi:10.1002/job.1894.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steenkamp, M. M., Suvak, M. K., Dickstein, B. D., Shea, M. T., & Litz, B. T. (2014). Emotional functioning in obsessive-compulsive personality disorder: Comparison to borderline personality disorder and healthy controls. Journal of Personality Disorders, 29(6), 794–808. doi:10.1521/pedi_2014_28_174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Surcinelli, P., Codispoti, M., Montebarocci, O., Rossi, N., & Baldaro, B. (2006). Facial emotion recognition in trait anxiety. Anxiety Disorders, 20, 110–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H., & Sullivan, K. (2000). A componential view of theory of mind: Evidence from Williams’s syndrome. Cognition, 76(1), 59–90. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00069-X.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tracy, J. L., & Randles, D. (2013). Four models of basic emotions: A review of Ekman and Cordaro, izard, Levenson, and Panksepp and watt. Emotion Review, 3(4), 397–4405. doi:10.1177/1754073911410747.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wai, M., & Tiliopoulos, N. (2012). The affective and cognitive empathic nature of the dark triad of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 52, 794–799. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2012.01.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1997). WAIS-III administration and scoring manual. San Antonio, Texas: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Widiger, T. A., & Trull, T. J. (2007). Plate tectonics in the classification of personality disorder: Shifting to a dimensional model. American Psychologist, 62(2), 71–83. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.62.2.71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zhao, H., Zhang, H., & Xu, Y. (2016). Does the dark triad of personality predict corrupt intention? The mediating role of belief in good luck. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 608. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00608.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was partially supported by the Fundação para a Ciência e para a Tecnologia (CBMR project: UID/BIM/04773/2013).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ana Teresa Martins.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

All the authors declare no conflict of interest

Ethical Approval

All the procedures performed in this study were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Martins, A.T., Ros, A., Valério, L. et al. Basic Emotion Recognition According to Clinical Personality Traits. Curr Psychol 38, 879–889 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-017-9661-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-017-9661-1

Keywords

Navigation