Skip to main content
Log in

Effects of Rumination Processing Modes on the Tendency to Avoid Failure

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Previous studies have shown that rumination has two processing modes: abstract processing mode and concrete processing mode. Abstract processing mode is associated with risk factors for depression. However, the effects of abstract processing mode on the tendency to avoid difficult situations, which is characteristic of major depression, are not clear. Our aim was to elucidate these effects, using laboratory-based methods. Thirty-four university students were recruited and divided into two group conditions abstract processing mode or concrete processing mode rumination. First, participants attempted a mathematics failure task. After the task, participants were interviewed in order to induce each processing mode. After the interview, they attempted the same task and were told that they could stop at any time. Results showed that the abstract processing mode rather than concrete processing mode group showed increased commitment for the second task. This indicated that rumination processing modes may increase the tendency to avoid difficult situations.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aoyagi, H., & Hosoda, K. (1994). Senkoukaiketsu fukanoukadai to kouzokukaiketsu kanoukadai no ruijisei ga kouzokukaiketsu kanoukadai no suikou ni oyobosu kouka [The effect of the similarity between the first unsolvable tasks and the second solvable or unsolvable ones upon the performance of test tasks]. Waseda journal of human sciences, 7, 33–41 Retrieved from http://dspace.wul.waseda.ac.jp/dspace/handle/2065/3881.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bassanini, A., Caselli, G., Fiore, F., Ruggiero, G. M., Sassaroli, S., & Watkins, E. R. (2014). Why "why" seems better than "how." Processes underlining repetitive thinking in an Italian non-clinical sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 64, 18–23. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2014.01.063.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck depression inventory (2nd ed.). San Antonio: Psychological Cooperation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brockmeyer, T., Holtfoth, M. G., Krieger, T., Altenstein, D., Doerig, N., Zimmermann, et al. (2015). Preliminary evidence for a nexus between rumination, behavioural avoidance, motive satisfaction and depression. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 22, 232–239. doi:10.1002/cpp.1885.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319–333. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eisma, M. C., Stroebe, M. S., Schut, H. A. W., Stroebe, W., Boelen, P. A., & van den Bout, J. (2013). Avoidance processes mediate the relationship between rumination and symptoms of complicated grief and depression following loss. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122, 961–970. doi:10.1037/a0034051.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eisma, M. C., Rinck, M., Stroebe, M. S., Schut, H. A. W., Boelen, P. A., Stroebe, W., & van den Bout, J. (2015). Rumination and implicit avoidance following bereavement: An approach avoidance task investigation. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 47, 84–91. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2014.11.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Giurgescu, C., Zenk, S. N., Templin, T. N., Engeland, C. G., Dancy, B. L., Park, C. G., et al. (2015). The impact of neighborhood environment, social support, and avoidance coping on depressive symptoms of pregnant african-american women. Women's Health Issues, 25, 294–302. doi:10.1016/j.whi.2015.02.001.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hao, L., Yang, J., Wang, Y., Zhang, S., Xie, P., Luo, Q., et al. (2015). Neural correlates of causal attribution in negative events of depressed patients: Evidence from an fMRI study. Clinical Neurophysiology, 126, 1331–1337. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2014.10.146.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hasegawa, A. (2013). Translation and initial validation of the Japanese version of the ruminative responses scale 1. Psychological Reports, 112, 716–726. doi:10.2466/02.08.PR0.112.3.716-726.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Ide, E., Leung, C., Kitayama, S., Takata, T., & Matsumoto, H. (2001). Divergent consequences of success and failure in Japan and north America: An investigation of self-improving motivations and malleable selves. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 599–615. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.4.599.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kingston, R. E. F., Watkins, E. R., & Nolen–Hoeksema, S. (2014). Investigating functional properties of depressive rumination: Insight and avoidance. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 5, 244–258. doi:10.5127/jep.038013.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kojima, M., & Furukawa, T. (2003). Nihongo ban Beck depression inventory, 2nd ed [Japanese version of the Beck depression inventory, 2nd ed.]. Tokyo: Nippon-Hyoron-sha Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krieger, T., & Altenstein, D., Baettig, I., Doerig, N., & Holtforth, M.G. (2013). Self-compassion in depression: Associations with depressive symptoms, rumination, and avoidance in depressed outpatients. Behavior Therapy, 44, 501–513. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2013.04.004.

  • Kuiper, N. (1978). Depression and causal attributions for success and failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 236–246. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.36.3.236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martel, C. R., Addis, M. E., & Jacobson N. S. (2001). Depression in context: Streategies for guided action ( Kumano, H. & Suzuki, S.Tras. & Eds.), New York: W. W. Norton & Company.

  • Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (1996). Some ruminative thoughts. In R. S. Wyer Jr. (Ed.), Ruminative thoughts (pp. 1–47). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moberly, N. J., & Watkins, E. R. (2006). Processing mode influences the relationship between trait rumination and emotional vulnerability. Behavior Therapy, 37, 281–291. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2006.02.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morrison, R., & O'Connor, R. C. (2008). A systematic review of the relationship between rumination and suicidality. Suicide & Life-Threatening Behavior, 38, 523–538.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moulds, M. L., Kandris, E., Starr, S., & Wong, A. C. M. (2007). The relationship between rumination, avoidance and depression in a non-clinical sample. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 251–261. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2006.03.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1991). A prospective study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: The 1989 Loma Prieta earthquake. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 115–121. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.61.1.115.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ottenbreit, N. D., & Dobson, K. S. (2004). Avoidance and depression: The construction of the cognitive-behavioral avoidance scale. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 293–313. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00140-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pearson, M., Brewin, C. R., Rhodes, J., & McCarron, G. (2008). Frequency and nature of rumination in chronic depression: A preliminary study. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 37, 160–169. doi:10.1080/16506070801919224.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peckham, A. D., McHugh, R. K., & Otto, M. W. (2010). A meta-analysis of the magnitude of biased attention in depression. Depression and Anxiety, 27, 1135–1142. doi:10.1002/da.20755.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raes, F., Watkins, E. R., Mark, J., Williams, G., & Hermans, D. (2008). Non-ruminative processing reduces overgeneral autobiographical memory retrieval in students. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 748–756. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2008.03.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rimes, K. A., & Watkins, E. (2005). The effects of self-focused ruminationon global negative self-judgements in depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 1673–1681. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2004.12.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rinck, M., & Becker, E. S. (2007). Approach and avoidance in fear of spiders. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38, 105–120. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2006.10.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stöber, J., & Borkovec, T. D. (2002). Reduced concreteness of worry in generalized anxiety disorder: Findings from a therapy study. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 26, 89–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Takahashi, Y., Yamagata, S., Kijima, N., Shigemasu, K., Ono, Y., & Ando, J. (2007). Gray no Kishitsu model―BIS/BAS syakudo nihongo ban no sakusei to souseijihou ni yoru koudou idengaku teki kentou [Gray’s temperament model: Development of Japanese version of BIS/BAS scales and a behavior genetic investigation using the twin methd]. The Japanese Journal of Personality, 15, 276–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Terada, M., & Ura, M. (2013). Kadaidouki to yuunoukan ni tyakumoku shita jikoseigyo mekanisumu no kentou[Examination of the self-control mechanism focusing on autonomous motivation and competence.] The Japanese Journal of Psychology, 84, 477–487.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 247–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, E. R. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 163–206. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.163.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, E. R., & Moberly, N. J. (2009). Concreteness training reduces dysphoria: A pilot proof-of-principle study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 48–53. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2008.10.014.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, E. R., & Moulds, M. (2005). Distinct modes of ruminative self-focus: Impact of abstract versus concrete rumination on problem solving in depression. Emotion, 5, 319–328. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2004.11.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, E., & Teasdale, J. D. (2001). Rumination and overgeneral memory in depression: Effects of self-focus and analytic thinking. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 353–357. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.110.2.333.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, E. R., Moberly, N. J., & Moulds, M. L. (2008). Processing mode causally influences emotional reactivity: Distinct effects of abstract versus concrete construal on emotional response. Emotion, 8, 364–378. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.8.3.364.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, E. R., Baeyens, C. B., & Read, R. (2009). Concreteness training reduces dysphoria: Proof-of-principle for repeated cognitive bias modification in depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 55–64. doi:10.1037/a0013642.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, E. R., Taylor, R. S., Byng, R., Baeyens, C. B., Read, R., Pearson, K., & Watson, L. (2012). Guided self-help concreteness training as an intervention for major depression in primary care: A randomized controlled trial. Psychological Medicine, 42, 1359–1371. doi:10.1017/S0033291711002480.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported in part by grant from the Hiroshima University Education and Research Support Foundation. Many thanks to Dr. Makoto Iwanaga and Yoshihiro Kanai for kindly contacting and providing the study material.

This study already submitted by master’s dissertation in Graduate school of Education, Hiroshima University.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kohei Kambara.

Ethics declarations

Funding

This study was funded by the Hiroshima University Education and Research Support Foundation.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Conflict of Interest

Author Kohei Kambara declares that he has no conflict of interest. Author Akiko Ogata declares that she has no conflict of interest, Author Yugo Kira declares that he has no conflict of interest.

Appendix

Appendix

Interview questions in abstract conditions

1

Why do you think that you solved the problem with such a solution?

2

What do you think is the reason why you got the results of this task?

3

Do you think that there are characteristic differences how you performed compared with others?

4

Why did you move your eyes during a problem like that?

5

Do you think that this problem has a distinct kind of meaning for you?

6

Why do you think that any thoughts floated during the task?

7

Why do you think that you moved your body during a problem like this?

Interview questions in concrete conditions

1

How much time did you take for each task problem?.

2

What you wrote in a problem booklet or draft that you provided?

3

Please explain to me the task precisely, and what kind of problem you solved, so that even a person who is not familiar with it can understand.

4

How did you move your eyes during the task as time went on?

5

What kind of method did you use to solve the task?

6

What kind of thoughts did you have during the task?

7

How did you move your body during the problem?

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kambara, K., Ogata, A. & Kira, Y. Effects of Rumination Processing Modes on the Tendency to Avoid Failure. Curr Psychol 38, 1204–1214 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-017-9655-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-017-9655-z

Keywords

Navigation