Skip to main content
Log in

Dependence on a Partner and Relationship Maintenance Effort: Experimentally Manipulated Dependence Promoted Ingratiation but Not Guilt

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Dependence on a partner facilitates various types of relationship maintenance effort. In this paper, we report on two experiments in which the level of dependence was manipulated. Study 1 tested whether dependence promotes other enhancement (a form of ingratiation, whereby the likeability of the partner is positively distorted). Study 2 tested whether dependence amplifies a sense of guilt after inadvertently committing a mild form of transgression against the partner, and whether amplified guilt facilitates self-punishment. In both experiments, dependence was manipulated in a conceptually similar manner: Participants’ experimental rewards were partially determined by their partner’s decision. Dependence promoted other enhancement (Study 1) but did not amplify guilt, and failed to promote self-punishment (Study 2). Possible reasons for the contradictory findings of the two studies are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The imbalance in the sample sizes is due to the original design of the experiment: originally, the intention was to investigate the targets’ resource allocation in the dictator game as well as the evaluators’ use of other enhancement in the first task. The no dependence condition consisted of two separate conditions associated with the dictator game. However, the dictator game yielded little difference across conditions for the ceiling effect. As the two conditions were completely identical in terms of how the evaluation task was conducted, we collapsed the two conditions into the no dependence condition in this report. No variables reported in the Result section differed significantly between the collapsed two conditions.

  2. The difference in the evaluators’ ratings may simply reflect some difference in the contents of the targets’ self-introduction essays. To preclude this interpretation, we had 17 new participants rate the essays with the same five items (i.e., the three other enhancement items and the two filler items). Each participant in this follow-up study rated about half of the essays. Since eight or nine participants rated each target’s essay, these new ratings were aggregated for each target. The ratings did not significantly differ as a function of the condition in the original study: 5.82 (SD = .90) and 5.94 (SD = .90) in the dependence and no dependence conditions, respectively, t(58) = 0.48, ns. Therefore, the difference in the evaluations in the original study is not attributable to possible differences in essay contents.

  3. Similar self-punishment experiments were previously conducted in Japan (e.g., Ohtsubo et al. 2014; Tanaka et al. 2015; Watanabe and Ohtsubo 2012). The materials and procedures used in Study 2 were adapted from these studies, and were readily applicable to the sample in Study 2 (i.e., Japanese undergraduate students).

References

  • Bastian, B., Jetten, J., & Fasoli, F. (2011). Cleansing the soul by hurting the flesh: The guilt-reducing effect of pain. Psychological Science, 22, 334–335. doi:10.1177/0956797610397058.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burnette, J. L., McCullough, M. E., Van Tongeren, D. R., & Davis, D. E. (2012). Forgiveness results from integrating information about relationship value and exploitation risk. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 345–356. doi:10.1177/014616721142458.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (2016). Do online social media cut through the constraints that limit the size of offline social networks? Royal Society Open Science, 3, 150292. doi:10.1098/rsos.150292.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fitzsimons, G. M., & Fishbach, A. (2010). Sifting closeness: Interpersonal effects of personal goal progress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 535–549. doi:10.1037/a0018581.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fitzsimons, G. M., & Shah, J. Y. (2008). How goal instrumentality shapes relationship evaluations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 319–337. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.95.2.319.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, R. A. (1996). Impact of ingratiation on judgments and evaluations: A meta-analytic investigation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 54–70. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.71.1.54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, C. A., Judge, T. A., & Ferris, G. R. (2003). Influence tactics and work outcomes: A meta-analysis. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 24, 89–106. doi:10.1002/job.181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inbar, Y., Pizzaro, D. A., Gilovich, T., & Ariely, D. (2013). Moral masochism: On the connection between guilt and self-punishment. Emotion, 13, 14–18. doi:10.1037/a0029749.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, E. E., & Pittman, T. S. (1982). Toward a general theory of strategic self-presentation. In J. Suls (Ed.), Psychological perspectives on the self (Vol. 1, pp. 231–262). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, E. E., Jones, R. G., & Gargen, K. J. (1963). Some conditions affecting the evaluation of a conformist. Journal of Personality, 31, 270–288. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1963.tb01855.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kipnis, D., & Vanderveer, R. (1971). Ingratiation and the use of power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17, 280–286. doi:10.1037/h0030587.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Main, K. J., Dahl, D. W., & Darke, P. R. (2007). Deliberative and automatic bases of suspicion: Empirical evidence of the sinister attribution error. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 17, 59–69. doi:10.1207/s15327663jcp1701_9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matsumura, A., & Ohtsubo, Y. (2012). Praise is reciprocated with tangible benefits: Social exchange between symbolic resources and concrete resources. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3, 250–256. doi:10.1177/1948550611417016.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCullough, M. E., Luna, L. R., Berry, J. W., Tabak, B. A., & Bono, G. (2010). On the form and function of forgiving: Modeling the time-forgiveness relationship and testing the valuable relationships hypothesis. Emotion, 10, 358–376. doi:10.1037/a0019349.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Milardo, R. M., Johnson, M. P., & Huston, T. L. (1983). Developing close relationships: Changing patterns of interaction between pair members and social networks. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 964–976. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.44.5.964.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miritello, G., Lara, R., Cebrian, M., & Moro, E. (2013). Limited communication capacity unveils strategies for human interaction. Scientific Reports, 3, article number 1950. doi:10.1038/srep01950.

  • Nelissen, R. M. A. (2012). Guilt-induced self-punishment as a sign of remorse. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3, 139–144. doi:10.1177/1948550611411520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelissen, R. M. A. (2014). Relational utility as a moderator of guilt in social interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106, 257–271. doi:10.1037/a0034711.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nelissen, R. M. A., & Zeelenberg, M. (2009). When guilt evokes self-punishment: Evidence for the existence of a dobby effect. Emotion, 9, 118–122. doi:10.1037/a0014540.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ohtsubo, Y., & Yagi, A. (2015). Relationship value promotes costly apology-making: Testing the valuable relationships hypothesis from the perpetrator’s perspective. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36, 232–239. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.11.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ohtsubo, Y., Matsunaga, M., Komiya, A., Tanaka, H., Mifune, N., & Yagi, A. (2014). Oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) polymorphism and self-punishment after an unintentional transgression. Personality and Individual Differences, 69, 182–186. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2014.05.033.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schlenker, B. R. (1980). Impression management: The self-concept, social identity, and interpersonal relations. Monterey: Brooks.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherman, D. K., & Cohen, G. L. (2006). The psychology of self-defense: Self-affirmation theory. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 38, pp. 183–242). San Diego: Academic Press. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(06)38004-5.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Sutcliffe, A., Dunbar, R., Binder, J., & Arrow, H. (2012). Relationships and the social brain: Integrating psychological and evolutionary perspectives. British Journal of Psychology, 103, 149–168. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02061.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tanaka, H., Yagi, A., Komiya, A., Mifune, N., & Ohtsubo, Y. (2015). Shame-prone people are more likely to punish themselves: A test of the reputation-maintenance explanation for self-punishment. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 9, 1–7. doi:10.1037/ebs0000016.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tangney, J. P., & Dearing, R. L. (2002). Shame and guilt. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Mashek, D. J. (2007). Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 345–372. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070145.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Vonk, R. (2002). Self-serving interpretation of flattery: Why ingratiation works. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 515–526. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.82.4.515.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watanabe, E., & Ohtsubo, Y. (2012). Costly apology and self-punishment after an unintentional transgression. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 10, 87–105. doi:10.1556/JEP.10.2012.3.1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yagi, A. (2016). The effect of relational utility on the severity of guilt in the context of new relationships. Japanese Journal of Interpersonal and Social Psychology, 16, 9–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeelenberg, M., & Breugelmans, S. M. (2008). The role of interpersonal harm in distinguishing regret from guilt. Emotion, 8, 589–596. doi:10.1037/a0012894.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zhu, R., Jin, T., Shen, X., Zhang, S., Mia, X., & Liu, C. (2017). Relational utility affects self-punishment in direct and indirect reciprocity situations. Social Psychology, 48, 19–27. doi:10.1027/1864-9335/a000291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Asami Kogire, Megumi Suzuki, and Haru Taniguchi for their assistance in conducting the research.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Yohsuke Ohtsubo.

Ethics declarations

Funding

This study was funded by the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (KAKENHI No. 21683006 & No. 26590132).

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Conflict of Interest

Yohsuke Ohtsubo declares that he has no conflict of interest. Ayano Yagi declares that she has no conflict of interest. Koji Kandori declares that he has no conflict of interest. Asami Matsumura declares that she has no conflict of interest.

Electronic supplementary material

ESM 1

(XLSX 16 kb)

ESM 2

(XLSX 19 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ohtsubo, Y., Yagi, A., Kandori, K. et al. Dependence on a Partner and Relationship Maintenance Effort: Experimentally Manipulated Dependence Promoted Ingratiation but Not Guilt. Curr Psychol 38, 676–683 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-017-9642-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-017-9642-4

Keywords

Navigation