Skip to main content
Log in

Toward a Non-memory Misinformation Effect: Accessing the Original Source Does Not Prevent Yielding to Misinformation

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The misinformation effect is regarded by many to be fully memory-related. The paper presents results demonstrating that it may occur without memory distortions or source monitoring errors. Three experiments were conducted, in which the participants were allowed to access the original and post-event source while answering. All experiments used a discrepancy detection test in order to see whether participants aware of discrepancies and having access to the original source, still provide answers consistent with misinformation. In Experiments 2 and 3, the participants’ actions in the modified procedure were recorded, ensuring that subjects actively access the original source. A misinformation effect was obtained in all experiments. Moreover, awareness of discrepancies did not fully protect against the misinformation effect. Results demonstrate that existing memory-related explanations of the misinformation effect, including source misattribution, may not be enough to fully understand it.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ayers, M. S., & Reder, L. M. (1998). A theoretical review of the misinformation effect: predictions from an activation-based memory model. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 5, 1–21. doi:10.3758/BF03209454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baym, C. J., & Gonsalves, B. D. (2010). Comparison of neural activity that leads to true memories, false memories, and forgetting: an fMRI study of the misinformation effect. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 10(3), 339–348. doi:10.3758/CABN.10.3.339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bekerian, D. A., & Bowers, J. N. (1983). Eyewitness testimony: were we misled? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 1, 139–145. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.9.1.139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belli, R. F. (1993). Failure of interpolated tests in inducing memory impairment with final modified tests: evidence unfavorable to the blocking hypothesis. American Journal of Psychology, 106, 407–427.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blank, H. (1998). Memory states and memory tasks. An integrative framework for eyewitness memory and suggestibility. Memory, 6, 481–529. doi:10.1080/741943086.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blank, H. (2005). Another look at retroactive and proactive interference: a quantitative analysis of conversion processes. Memory, 13(2), 200–224.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C. J., & Kingma, J. (1984). Do children have to remember to reason? A fuzzy trace theory of transitivity development. Developmental Review, 4, 311–377. doi:10.1016/0273-2297(84)90021-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R. M. J. (1989). Suppressing valid inferences with conditionals. Cognition, 31, 61–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chechile, R. A. (2006). Memory hazard functions: a vehicle for theory development and test. Psychological Review, 113(1), 31–56. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.113.1.31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., & Goldstein, D. G. (1996). Reasoning the fast and frugal way. Models of bounded rationality. Psychological Review, 103, 650–669. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.103.4.650.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Giles, J. W., Gopnik, A., & Heyman, G. D. (2002). Source monitoring reduces the suggestibility of preschool children. Psychological Science, 13, 288–291. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00453.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, E., Flynn, M. S., & Loftus, E. F. (1982). Inducing resistance to misleading information. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 207–219. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(82)90571-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hupbach, A., Gomez, R., & Nadel, L. (2009). Episodic memory reconsolidation: updating or source confusion? Memory, 17, 502–510. doi:10.1080/09658210902882399.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lindsay, D. S. (1990). Misleading suggestions can impair eyewitness’s ability to remember event details. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 1077–1083. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.16.6.1077.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindsay, D. S., & Johnson, M. K. (1989). The eyewitness suggestibility effect and memory for source. Memory & Cognition, 17, 349–358. doi:10.3758/BF03198473.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loftus, E. F. (2005). Planting misinformation in the human mind: a 30-year investigation of the malleability of memory. Learning & Memory, 12, 361–366. doi:10.1101/lm.94705.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loftus, E. F., & Hoffman, H. G. (1989). Misinformation and memory: the creation of new memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 118, 100–104. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.118.1.100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loftus, E. F., & Palmer, J. C. (1974). Reconstruction of automobile destruction: an example of the interaction between language and memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 585–589. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(74)80011-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loftus, E. F., Miller, D. G., & Burns, H. J. (1978). Semantic integration of verbal information into a visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 19–31. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.4.1.19.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCloskey, M., & Zaragoza, M. S. (1985a). Misleading postevent information and memory for events: arguments and evidence against memory impairment hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 1–16. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.114.1.1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCloskey, M., & Zaragoza, M. S. (1985b). Postevent information and memory: reply to Loftus, Schooler, and Wagenaar. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 381–387. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.114.3.381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe Eich, J. (1982). A composite holographic associative recall model. Psychological Review, 89, 627–661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, K. J., & Zaragoza, M. S. (1996). Repeated exposure to suggestion and fales memory: the role of contextual variablity. Journal of Memory and Language, Special Issue: Illusions of Memory, 35, 246–260. doi:10.1006/jmla.1996.0014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oeberst, A., & Blank, H. (2012). Undoing suggestive influence on memory: the reversibility of the eyewitness misinformation effect. Cognition, 125, 141–159. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.07.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Orne, M. T. (1962). On the social psychology of the psychological experiment: with particular reference to demand characteristics and their implications. American Psychologist, 17, 776–783.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plous, S. (1993). The psychology of judgment and decision making. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polczyk, R. (2007). Mechanizmy efektu dezinformacji w kontekście zeznań swiadka naocznego [Mechanisms of the misinformation effect in the context of eyewitness testimonies]. Kraków: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Jagiellońskiego.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saunders, J., & MacLeod, M. D. (2002). New evidence on the suggestibility of memory: the role of retrieval-induced forgetting in misinformation effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 127–142. doi:10.1037/1076-898X.8.2.127.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Szpitalak, M., & Polczyk, R. (2011). Distortions in eyewitness memory – memory and non-memory mechanisms. Problems of Forensic Sciences, 85, 40–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tousignant, J. P., Hall, D., & Loftus, E. F. (1986). Discrepancy detection and vulnerability to misleading postevent information. Memory and Cognition, 14, 329–338. doi:10.3758/BF03202511.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. Oxford: Clandon Press/Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zaragoza, M. S., & Koshmider, J. W., III. (1989). Misled subjects may know more than their performance implies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 246–255. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.15.2.246.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zaragoza, M. S., & Lane, S. M. (1994). Source misattributions and the suggestibility of eyewitness memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 20, 934–945. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.20.4.934.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zaragoza, M. S., McCloskey, M., & Jamis, M. (1987). Misleading postevent information and recall of the original event: further evidence against the memory impairment hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 36–44. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.13.1.36.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mateusz Polak.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Polak, M., Dukała, K., Szpitalak, M. et al. Toward a Non-memory Misinformation Effect: Accessing the Original Source Does Not Prevent Yielding to Misinformation. Curr Psychol 35, 1–12 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-015-9352-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-015-9352-8

Keywords

Navigation