Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Everyday Activities: Beneficial Effects of Eudaimonic and Hedonic Motivation on Subjective Well-Being

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The study focuses on the impact of time and motives for everyday activities on mood and daily satisfaction. One hundred twenty-one undergraduate students participated in study using experience sampling method. The short PANAS scale, one item momentary satisfaction measure and state orientation to happiness scale were used. The measures were administered five times a day for 7 days via hand-held devices. The data were modeled using a linear mixed-effects approach. The models fitted the data reasonably well, especially for satisfaction and positive affect (PA) sharing similar parameter estimates. Both showed a nonlinear increase through the week while PA showed a similarly shaped circa-diurnal rhythm too. Negative affect (NA) showed almost no rhythm at all. Both hedonic and eudaimonic motives increased satisfaction and PA. Hedonic motives had a negative impact on NA. Generally, on momentary level, hedonic motives have a greater impact while longer intervals seem to emphasize the importance of eudaimonic motives.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baayen, H. R., & Milin, P. (2010). Analyzing reaction times. International Journal of Psychological Research, 3(2), 12–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–278. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, L. F., & Barrett, D. J. (2001). An introduction to computerized experience sampling in psychology. Social Science Computer Review, 19(2), 175–185. doi:10.1177/089443930101900204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, D. M., Maechler, M., & Bolker, B. (2013). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. Retrieved from http://lme4.r-forge.r-project.org/.

  • Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2002). The pursuit of meaningfulness in life. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 608–618). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolger, N., Davis, A., & Rafaeli, E. (2003). Diary methods: capturing life as it is lived. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 579–616. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145030.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Compton, W. C., Smith, M. L., Cornish, K. A., & Qualls, D. L. (1996). Factor structure of mental health measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 406–413. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.406.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crawley, M. J. (2007). The R book (p. 877). Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1991). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience (p. 336). New York: Harper Perennial.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997). Creativity: Flow and the psychology of discovery and invention (p. 464). New York: Harper Perennial.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Hunter, J. (2003). Happiness in everyday life: the uses of experience sampling. Journal of Happiness Studies, 4, 185–199. doi:10.1023/A:1024409732742.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1987). Validity and reliability of the experience-sampling method. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175(9), 526–536. doi:10.1097/00005053-198709000-00004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2006). Hedonia, eudaimonia, and well-being: an introduction. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9(1), 1–11. doi:10.1007/s10902-006-9018-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Delle Fave, A., Brdar, I., Freire, T., Vella-Brodrick, D., & Wissing, M. P. (2010). The eudaimonic and hedonic components of happiness: qualitative and quantitative findings. Social Indicators Research, 100(2), 185–207. doi:10.1007/s11205-010-9632-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Delle Fave, A., Massimini, F., & Bassi, M. (2011). Psychological selection and optimal experience across cultures. In A. Delle Fave, F. Massimini, & M. Bassi (Eds.), Psychological selection and optimal experience across cultures: Social empowerment through personal growth (Vol. 2, pp. 3–18). Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands. doi:10.1007/978-90-481-9876-4.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E. (1994). Assessing subjective well-being: progress and opportunities. Social Indicators Research, 31(2), 103–157. doi:10.1007/BF01207052.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E. (2009). The Science of Well-Being. (E. Diener, Ed.) (Vol. 37, pp. 11–58). Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands. doi:10.1007/978-90-481-2350-6.

  • Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being: three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125(2), 276–302. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.2.276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: the broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56(3), 218–226. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.56.3.218.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Golder, S. A., & Macy, M. W. (2011). Diurnal and seasonal mood vary with work, sleep, and daylength across diverse cultures. Science (New York, N.Y.), 333(6051), 1878–1881. doi:10.1126/science.1202775.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, L. W., Knight, T., & Richardson, B. (2013). The hedonic and eudaimonic validity of the orientations to happiness scale. Social Indicators Research, 115(3), 1087–1099. doi:10.1007/s11205-013-0264-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huta, V. (2005). Pursuing pleasure versus growth and excellence : Links with different aspects of well-being. Montreal: McGill University. Retrieved from http://digitool.library.mcgill.ca/R/-?func=dbin-jump-full&object_id=85919&silo_library=GEN01.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huta, V. (2011). Linking peoples’ pursuit of eudaimonia and hedonia with characteristics of their parents: parenting styles, verbally endorsed values, and role modeling. Journal of Happiness Studies, 13(1), 47–61. doi:10.1007/s10902-011-9249-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huta, V. (2013). Eudaimonia. In S. David, I. Boniwell, & A. C. Ayers (Eds.), Oxford handbook of happiness (pp. 201–213). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huta, V., & Ryan, R. M. (2009). Pursuing pleasure or virtue: the differential and overlapping well-being benefits of hedonic and eudaimonic motives. Journal of Happiness Studies, 11(6), 735–762. doi:10.1007/s10902-009-9171-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., Diener, E., & Schwarz, N. (1999). Well-being: Foundations of hedonic psychology (p. 608). New York: Russell Sage Foundation Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kardum, I. (1998). Affect intensity and frequency: their relation to mean level and variability of positive and negative affect and Eysencks personality traits. Personality and Individual Differences, 26(1), 33–47. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00157-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kashdan, T. B., Biswas-Diener, R., & King, L. A. (2008). Reconsidering happiness: the costs of distinguishing between hedonics and eudaimonia. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 3(4), 219–233. doi:10.1080/17439760802303044.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keyes, C. L. M., Shmotkin, D., & Ryff, C. D. (2002). Optimizing well-being: the empirical encounter of two traditions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(6), 1007–1022. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.1007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kraut, R. (1979). Two conceptions of happines. Philosophical Review, 88(2), 167–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, R. J. (2009). The contribution of positive and negative affect to emotional well-being. Psychological Topics, 18, 247–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larson, R., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1983). The experience sampling method. In H. Reis (Ed.), New directions for naturalistic methods in the behavioral sciences (pp. 41–56). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyubomirsky, S., Sheldon, K. M., & Schkade, D. (2005). Pursuing happiness: the architecture of sustainable change. Review of General Psychology, 9(2), 111–131. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.9.2.111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGregor, I., & Little, B. R. (1998). Personal projects, happiness, and meaning: on doing well and being yourself. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(2), 494–512. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.2.494.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Norton, D. L. (1976). Personal destinies: A philosophy of ethical individualism. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oishi, S., Diener, E., Suh, E., & Lucas, R. E. (1999). Value as a moderator in subjective well-being. Journal of Personality, 67(1), 157–184. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.00051.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C., Park, N., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2005). Orientations to happiness and life satisfaction: the full life versus the empty life. Journal of Happiness Studies, 6(1), 25–41. doi:10.1007/s10902-004-1278-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinheiro, J. C., & Bates, D. M. (2000). Mixed-effects models in S and S-PLUS (p. 528). New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • R Core Team. (2013). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna. Retrieved from http://www.r-project.org/.

  • Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. The American Psychologist, 55(1), 68–78. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11392867.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: a review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 141–166. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.141.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schimmack, U. (2002). Methodological issues in the assessment of the affective component of subjective well-being. In A. Ohn & M. van Dulmen (Eds.), Handbook of methods in positive psychology (pp. 96–110). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schimmack, U., Diener, E., & Oishi, S. (2002). Life-satisfaction is a momentary judgment and a stable personality characteristic: the use of chronically accessible and stable sources. Journal of Personality, 70(3), 345–384. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.05008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schimmack, U., & Oishi, S. (2005). The influence of chronically and temporarily accessible information on life satisfaction judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(3), 395–406. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.3.395.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schueller, S. M., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2010). Pursuit of pleasure, engagement, and meaning: relationships to subjective and objective measures of well-being. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 5(4), 253–263. doi:10.1080/17439761003794130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Authentic happiness: Using the new positive psychology to realize your potential for lasting fulfillment. New York: The Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M. E. P., Parks, A. C., & Steen, T. (2004). A balanced psychology and a full life. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 359(1449), 1379–1381. doi:10.1098/rstb.2004.1513.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Steger, M. F., Kashdan, T. B., & Oishi, S. (2008). Being good by doing good: daily eudaimonic activity and well-being. Journal of Research in Personality, 42(1), 22–42. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2007.03.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Streiner, D. L. (2003). Starting at the beginning: an introduction to coefficient alpha and internal consistency. Journal of Personality Assessment, 80(1), 99–103. doi:10.1207/S15327752JPA8001_18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vittersø, J. (2003). Flow versus life satisfaction: a projective use of cartoons to illustrate the difference between the evaluation approach and the intrinsic motivation approach to subjective quality of life. Journal of Happiness Studies, 4(2), 141–167. doi:10.1023/A:1024413112234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vittersø, J. (2004). Subjective well-being versus self-actualization: using the flow-simplex to promote a conceptual clarification of subjective quality of life. Social Indicators Research, 65(3), 299–331. doi:10.1023/B:SOCI.0000003910.26194.ef.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vittersø, J., Oelmann, H. I., & Wang, A. L. (2009a). Life satisfaction is not a balanced estimator of the good life: evidence from reaction time measures and self-reported emotions. Journal of Happiness Studies, 10(1), 1–17. doi:10.1007/s10902-007-9058-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vittersø, J., & Søholt, Y. (2011). Life satisfaction goes with pleasure and personal growth goes with interest: further arguments for separating hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 6(4), 326–335. doi:10.1080/17439760.2011.584548.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vittersø, J., Søholt, Y., Hetland, A., Thoresen, I. A., & Røysamb, E. (2009b). Was Hercules happy? Some answers from a functional model of human well-being. Social Indicators Research, 95(1), 1–18. doi:10.1007/s11205-009-9447-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waterman, A. S. (1993). Two conceptions of happiness: contrasts of personal expressiveness (eudaimonia) and hedonic enjoyment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(4), 678–691. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.64.4.678.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waterman, A. S. (2007). On the importance of distinguishing hedonia and eudaimonia when contemplating the hedonic treadmill. The American Psychologist, 62(6), 612–613. doi:10.1037/0003-066X62.6.612.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Waterman, A. S. (2008). Reconsidering happiness: a eudaimonist’s perspective. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 3(4), 234–252. doi:10.1080/17439760802303002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waterman, A. S., Schwartz, S. J., & Conti, R. (2008). The implications of two conceptions of happiness (Hedonic Enjoyment and Eudaimonia) for the understanding of intrinsic motivation. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9(1), 41–79. doi:10.1007/s10902-006-9020-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zelenski, J. M., & Larsen, R. J. (2000). The distribution of basic emotions in everyday life: a state and trait perspective from experience sampling data. Journal of Research in Personality, 34(2), 178–197. doi:10.1006/jrpe.1999.2275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Croatian Ministry of Science, Education and Sports under Grant 009-0342618-2193 and by the University of Rijeka under Grant 13.04.1.3.05.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marko Tončić.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tončić, M., Anić, P. Everyday Activities: Beneficial Effects of Eudaimonic and Hedonic Motivation on Subjective Well-Being. Curr Psychol 34, 644–653 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-014-9277-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-014-9277-7

Keywords

Navigation