Skip to main content
Log in

RETRACTED ARTICLE: Considering the Relationship Between Language, Culture and Cognition to Scrutinize the Lexical Influences on Cognition

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

This article was retracted on 14 June 2012

Abstract

In this paper, we examine the interrelationships among language, culture, and cognition. The central notion that individuals with different linguistic and cultural backgrounds think differently is not far from our everyday experience. If you have had the opportunity to engage in a conversation with a person whose native language is not English, you may have found that communication breaks down at times and that some concepts are not easily translated into another language. Or if you happen to be a fluent bilingual or multilingual, you may agree with those bilinguals or multilinguals who mention that they think differently in each of their language. A number of intriguing questions arise here. Is there a particular style of thinking that is natural for speakers of each language? If so, is it possible for a person to think in a different way, one that is not natural for that individual? Is this style of thinking imparted by the language, the culture, or both? These and lots of other questions have engaged the attention of anthropologists, linguists and psychologists, but the point that we are going to explore is the lexical influences on cognition considering the relationship between language, culture and cognition.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Agar, M. (1994). Language shock: Understanding the culture of conversation. New York: Quill / William Morrow.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baghramian, M. (2004). Relativism. London: Routledge.

  • Berlin, B., & Kay, P. (1969). Basic color terms: Their universality and evolution. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does language shape thought? Mandarin and English speakers’ conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology, 43, 1–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bowerman, M. (1996). Learning how to structure space for language: a crosslinguistic perspective. In P. Bloom, M. A. Petersen, L. Nadel, & M. F. Garrett (Eds.), Language and space (pp. 385–436). Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowerman, M., & Choi, S. (2001). Shaping meanings for language: universal and language-specific in the acquisition of spatial semantic categories. In M. Bowerman & S. C. Levinson (Eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development (pp. 475–511). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. (1958). Words and things. Glencoe: Free.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. W., & Lenneberg, E. H. (1954). A study in language and cognition. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 49, 454–462.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. B. (Ed.). (1956). Language, thought, and reality: Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Choi, S., & Bowerman, M. (1991). Learning to express motion events in English and Korean: the influence of language-specific lexicalization patterns. Cognition, 41, 83–121.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Choi, S., McDonough, L., Bowerman, M., & Mandler, J. M. (1999). Early sensitivity to language specific spatial categories in English and Korean. Cognitive Development, 14, 241–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clancy, P. M. (1985). The acquisition of Japanese. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition: Vol. 1. The data (pp. 373–524). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H. (1973). Space, time, semantics, and the child. In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language (pp. 27–63). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H., & Clark, E. V. (1977). Psychology and language: An introduction to psycholinguistics. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crystal, D. (1997). English as a global language. Cambridge: CUP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Devitt, M., & Sterelny, K. (1999). Language and reality: An introduction to the philosophy of language. Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, N. C., & Hennelly, R. A. (1980). A bilingual word-length effect: implications for intelligence testing and the relative ease of mental calculation in Welsh and English. British Journal of Psychology, 71, 43–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feist, M. I., & Gentner, D. (2001). An influence of spatial language on recognition memory for spatial scenes. Proceedings of the Twenty-third Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 279–284.

  • Fox, T. (2001). TEFL as imperialism or empowerment? Hwa Kang Journal of TEFL, 7, 137–150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuson, K. C., Smith, S. T., & Lo Cicero, A. M. (1997). Supporting Latino first grades’ ten-structured thinking in urban classrooms. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 28, 738–766.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Cauley, K. M., & Gordon, L. (1987). The eyes have it: lexical and syntactic comprehension in a new paradigm. Journal of Child Language, 14, 23–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A. (2001). Theories, language, and culture: Whorf without wincing. In M. Bowerman & S. C. Levinson (Eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development (pp. 45–69). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A., & Choi, S. (1990). Do linguistic differences lead to cognitive differences? A crosslinguistic study of semantic and cognitive development. First Language, 10, 199–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A., & Choi, S. (1995). Names, relational words, and cognitive development in English and Korean speakers: nouns are not always learned before verbs. In M. Tomasello & W. E. Merriman (Eds.), Beyond names for things: Young children’s acquisition of verbs (pp. 63–80). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A., Choi, S., & Baumberger, T. (1996). Crosslinguistic differences in early semantic and cognitive development. Cognitive Development, 11, 197–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heider, E. R. (1972). Universals in color naming and memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 93, 10–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hespos, S. J., & Spelke, E. S. (2004). Conceptual precursors to language. Nature, 430, 453–456.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hockett, C. F. (1954). Chinese versus English: an exploration of the Whorfian theses. In H. Hoijer (Ed.), Language in culture (pp. 106–123). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, J. (2001). An introduction to sociolinguistics (2nd ed.). London: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoosain, R. (1986). Language, orthography, and cognitive processes: Chinese perspectives for the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 9, 507–525.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoosain, R., & Salili, F. (1987). Language differences in pronunciation speed for numbers, digit span, and mathematical ability. Psychologia, 30, 34–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hurford, J. R. (1987). Language and number. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kay, P., & Kempton, W. (1984). What is the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis? American Anthropologist, 86, 65–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kay, P., & Regier, T. (2003). Resolving the question of color naming universals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 100, 9085–9089.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kay, P., & Regier, T. (2006). Language, thought, and color: recent developments. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 51–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keller, C., & Keller, J. (1996). Imaging in iron or thought is not inner speech. In J. J. Gumperz & S. Levinson (Eds.), Rethinking linguistic relativity (pp. 115–129). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Lenneberg, E. H. (1953). Cognition in ethnolinguistics. Language, 29, 463–471.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, S. C. (1996). Frames of reference and Molyneux’s question: crosslinguistic evidence. In P. Bloom, M. A. Peterson, L. Nadel, & M. F. Garrett (Eds.), Language and space (pp. 109–169). Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, S. C. (2001). Covariation between spatial language and cognition, and its implications for language learning. In M. Bowerman & S. C. Levinson (Eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development (pp. 566–588). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, S. (2003). Space in language and cognition: Explorations in cognitive diversity. Cambridge: CUP.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lucy, J. A. (1992a). Grammatical categories and cognition: A case study of the linguistic relativity hypothesis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lucy, J. A. (1992b). Language diversity and thought: A reformulation of the linguistic relativity hypothesis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lucy, J. A., & Shweder, R. A. (1979). Whorf and his critics: linguistic and nonlinguistic influences on color memory. American Anthropologist, 81, 581–615.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, L. (1986). “Eskimo words for snow”: a case study in the genesis and decay of an anthropological example. American Anthropologist, 88, 418–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, K. F., Smith, C. M., Zhu, J., & Zhang, H. (1995). Preschool origins of cross-national differences in mathematical competence: the role of number naming systems. Psychological Science, 6, 56–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miura, I. T. (1987). Mathematics achievement as a function of language. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 79–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miura, I. T., & Okamoto, Y. (1989). Comparisons of U.S. and Japanese first grades’ cognitive representation of number and understanding of place value. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 109–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miura, I. T., Kim, C. C., Chang, C.-M., & Okamoto, Y. (1988). Effects of language characteristics on children’s cognitive representation of number: cross-national comparisons. Child Development, 59, 1445–1450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pederson, E., Danziger, E., Wilkins, D., Levinson, S., Kita, S., & Senft, G. (1998). Semantic typology and spatial conceptualization. Language, 74, 557–589.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children (M. Cook, Trans.). New York: International Universities Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Pullum, G. K. (1991). The great Eskimo vocabulary hoax and other irreverent essays on the study of language. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberson, D., Davies, I. R. L., & Davidoff, J. (2000). Color categories are not universal: replications and new evidence from a Stone-Age culture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 369–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roberson, D., Davidoff, J., Davies, I. R. L., & Shapiro, L. R. (2005). Color categories: evidence for the cultural relativity hypothesis. Cognitive Psychology, 50, 378–411.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E. H. (1973). On the internal structure of perceptual and semantic categories. In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and acquisition of language (pp. 111–144). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwanenflugel, P. J., Blount, B. G., & Lin, P.-J. (1991). Cross-cultural aspects of word meanings. In P. J. Schwanenflugel (Ed.), The psychology of word meanings (pp. 71–90). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. (1996). From “thought and language” to “thinking for speaking”. In J. J. Gumperz & S. C. Levinson (Eds.), Rethinking linguistic relativity (pp. 70–96). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, H. W., Lee, S.-Y., & Stigler, J. W. (1986). Mathematics achievement of Chinese, Japanese, and American children. Science, 231, 693–699.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Varley, R. A., Klessinger, N. J. C., Romanowski, C. A. J., & Siegal, M. (2005). Agrammatic but numerate. Proceedings of the Natioanl Academy of Science, 102, 3519–3524.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whorf, B. L. (1956a). Language, thought, and reality: Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whorf, B. L. (1956b). Science and linguistics. In J. B. Carroll (Ed.), Language, thought, and reality: Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf (pp. 208–219). Cambridge: MIT. [Originally published 1940.].

    Google Scholar 

  • Zipf, G. K. (1935). The psycho-biology of language: An introduction to dynamic philology. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Iman Tohidian.

Additional information

This article has been retracted due to copyright violation.

The retraction note to this article can be found online at http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12144-012-9143-4.

About this article

Cite this article

Tohidian, I., Mir Tabatabaie, S.M. RETRACTED ARTICLE: Considering the Relationship Between Language, Culture and Cognition to Scrutinize the Lexical Influences on Cognition. Curr Psychol 29, 52–70 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-010-9072-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-010-9072-z

Keywords

Navigation