Skip to main content
Log in

When does giving voice or not matter? Procedural fairness effects as a function of closeness of reference points

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present study examined whether manipulating the closeness of reference points can provide further insights into explaining why people care so much about receiving voice (i.e., the opportunity to express one’s opinion with respect to allocation decisions). Participants read a scenario portraying a situation where they had always been a member of the relevant team (i.e., distant referent point condition) or where they had just become a member of the relevant team (i.e., close referent point condition). Thereafter, they were either told that they would receive voice or no voice with respect to the issue of distributing a financial bonus. The results showed that people cared more about voice when they were placed in the distant referent condition rather than in the close referent condition. This effect was strongest on participants’ positive emotions (i.e., being positive when receiving voice vs. receiving no voice) than on their negative emotions. The findings are discussed in light of procedural fairness, counterfactual thinking, and emotion literature.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baumeister, R.F., & Leafy, M.R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brockner, J., Heuer, L., Siegel, P.A., Wiesenfeld, B., Martin, C., Grover, S., Reed, T., & Bjorgvinsson, S. (1998). The moderating effect of self-esteem in reaction to voice: Converging evidence from five studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 394–407.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brockner, J., & Wiesenfeld, B.M. (1996). An integrative framework for explaining reactions to decisions: Interactive effects of outcomes and procedures. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 189–208.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cropanzano, R., & Folger, R. (1989). Referent cognitions and task decision autonomy: Beyond equity theory. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 293–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cropanzano, R., & Randall, M.L. (1995). Advance notions as a means of reducing relative deprivation. Social Justice Research, 8, 217–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, C.G., Lehman, D.R., Wortman, C.B., Silver, R.C., & Thompson, S.C. (1995). The undoing of traumatic life events. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 109–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Folger, R. (1977). Distributive and procedural justice: Combined impact of “voice” and improvement of experienced inequity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 108–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Folger, R. (1986). Rethinking equity theory: A referent cognitions model. In M. Bierhoff, R.L. Cohen, & J. Greenberg (Eds.), Justice in social relations (pp. 145–162). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilovich, T., & Medvec, V.H. (1994). The temporal pattern to the experience of regret. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 357–365.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, J. (1987). Using diaries to promote procedural justice in performance appraisals. Social Justice Research, 1, 219–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E.T., Friedman, R.S., Hadow, R.E., Idson, L.C., Ayduk, O.N., & Taylor, A. (2001). Achievement orientation from subjective histories of success: Promotion pride versus prevention. European Journal of Social Psychology, 31, 3–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Miller, D.T. (1986). Norm theory: Comparing reality to its alternatives, Psychological Review, 93, 136–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. 1982). The simulation heuristic. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky (Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 201–208). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koper, G., van Knippenberg, D., Bouhuijs, F., Vermunt, R., & Wilke, H.A.M. (1993). Procedural fairness and self-esteem. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 504–516.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landman, J. (1993). Regret: Persistence of the possible. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lind, E.A., Kanfer, R., & Earley, P.C. (1990). Voice, control, and procedural justice: Instrumental and noninstrumental concerns in fairness judgments, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 952–959.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lind, E.A., & Tyler, T.R. (1988). The social psychology of procedural justice. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markman, K.D., Gavanski, I., Sherman, S.J., & McMullen, M.N. (1993). The mental simulation of better and worse possible words. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 29, 87–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McFarlin, D.B., & Sweeney, P.D. (1996). Does having a say matter only if you get your way? Instrumental and value expressive effects of employee voice. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 18, 289–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McMullen, M. (1997). Affective contrast and assimilation in counterfactual thinking. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 77–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moorman, R.H. (199l). Relationship between organizational justice and organizational citizenship behaviors: Do fairness perceptions influence employee citizenship? Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 845–855.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nasco, S.A., & Marsh, K.L. (1999). Gaining control through counterfactual thinking. Personality, and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 556–568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roese, N.J. (1994). The functional basis of counteffactual thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 805–818.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roese, N.J. (1997). Counterfactual thinking. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 133–148.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sanna, L.J., Turley-Ames, K.J., & Meier, S. (1999). Mood, self-esteem, and simulated alternatives: Thought-provoking affective influences on counterfactual direction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 543–558.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thibaut, J.W., & Walker, L. (1975). Procedural justice: A psychological analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, T.R. (1989). The psychology of procedural justice: A test of the group value model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 333–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, T.R. (1990). Why people obey the law: Procedural justice, legitimacy, and compliance. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, T.R., & Lind, E.A. (1992). A relational model of authority in groups. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 115–191). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van den Bos, K. (1999). What are we talking about when we talk about no-voice procedures? On the psychology of the fair outcome effect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 560–577.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van den Bos, K., Lind, E. A., Vermunt, R., & Wilke, H.A.M. (1997). How do I judge my outcome when I do not know the outcome of others? The psychology of the fair process effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1034–1046.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van den Bos, K., & Miedema, J. (2000). Toward understanding why fairness matters: The influence of mortality salience on reactions to procedural fairness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 355–366.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van den Bos, K., & van Prooijen, J.-W. (2001). Referent cognitions theory: The role of closeness of reference points in the psychology of voice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 616–4526.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van den Bos, K., Vermunt, R., & Wilke, H.A.M. (1996). The consistency rule and the voice effect: The influence of expectations on procedural fairness judgements and performance. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 411–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van den Bos, K., Wilke, H.A.M., & Lind, E.A. (1998). When do we need procedural fairness? The role of trust in authority. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1449–1458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeelenberg, M., van Dijk, W.W., & Manstaed, A.S.R. (1998). Reconsidering the relation between regret and responsibility. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 74, 254–272.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

De Cremer, D., Stouten, J. When does giving voice or not matter? Procedural fairness effects as a function of closeness of reference points. Curr Psychol 24, 203–213 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-005-1022-9

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-005-1022-9

Keywords

Navigation