Skip to main content
Log in

Right Hemisphere Positivity Bias in Preconscious Processing: Data From Five Experiments

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Using a signal detection paradigm, participants’ sensitivity to emotionally toned stimuli was evaluated in five experiments. A tachistoscope was used to present stimuli to one hemisphere at a time, too rapidly for conscious identification. Pictures, words, and faces were pretested for emotional valence and familiarity. Stimulus selection was based on obtaining the largest possible difference between the positive and negative valence ratings, keeping familiarity equal. Each experiment used a 2×2×2 mixed groups design. The within-subject variables were the hemispheric presentation (right and left) and the emotional tone of the stimuli (positive and negative). Gender of the participant was the between-subjects variable. The results of these studies indicate greater sensitivity in the right than in the left hemisphere, and greater sensitivity to positive than to negative stimuli. Interpretations focus on the automatization of affect and the brain lateralization of emotional processing.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abrams, R.L., & Greenwald, A.G. (2000). Parts outweigh the whole (word) in unconscious analysis of meaning. Psychological Science, 11 (2), 118–124.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J.A. (1988). Automatic information processing: Implications for communication and affect. In L. Donohew, H.E. Sypher, & E.T. Higgens (Eds.). Communication, social cognition, and affect (pp. 9–32). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J.A. (1992). The ecology of automaticity: Toward establishing the conditions needed to produce automatic processing effects. American Journal of Psychology, 105, (2), 181–199.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J.A., Chaiken, S., Govender, R., & Pratto, F. (1992). The generality of the automatic attitude activation effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 893–912.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J.A., & Chartrand, T.L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54 (7), 462–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J.A., Litt, J., Pratto, F., & Spielman, L.A. (1989). On the preconscious evaluation of social stimuli. In A.F. Bennett & K.M. McConkey (Eds.) Cognition in Individual and Social Contexts. North Holland: Elsevier Science Publishers B.V.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beaton, A. (1985). Left side right side: A review of laterality research. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bomstein, R. (1989). Exposure and affect: Overview and meta-analysis of research, 1968-1987. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 265–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borod, J.C., Andelman, F., Obier, L.K., Tweedy, J.R., & Welkowitz, J. (1992). Right hemisphere specialization for the identification of emotional words and sentences: Evidence from stroke patients. Neuropsychologia, 30 (9), 827–844.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brody, L.R., & Hall, J.A. (1993). Gender and emotion. In Lewis, M. & Haviland, J.M. (Eds.) Handbook of Emotions. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J.T., & Berntson, G.G. (1999). The affect system: Architecture and operating characteristics. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8 (5), 133–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J.T., Gardner, W.L., & Berntson, G.G. (1999). The affect system has parallel and integrative processing components: Form follows function. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76 (5), 839–855.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J.T., & Petty, R.E. (1981). Lateral symmetry in the expression of cognition and emotion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 333–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Canli, T., Zhao, Z., Desmond, J.E., Kang, E., Gross, J., & Gabrieli, J.D. (2001). An fMRI study of personality influences on brain reactivity to emotional stimuli. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115 (1), 33–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carretie, L., Mercado, F., Tapia, M., & Hinojosa, J.A. (2001). Emotion, attention and the “negativity bias,” studied through event-related potentials. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 41 (1), 75–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cheesman, J., & Merikle, P.M. (1986). Distinguishing conscious from unconscious perceptual processes. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 40, 343–367.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dahl, M. (2001). Asymmetries in the processing of emotionally valenced words. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 42 (2), 97–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R.J. (1993). The neuropsychology of emotion and affective style. In Lewis, M. & Haviland, J.M. (Eds.). Handbook of Emotions. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R.J. (1995). Cerebral asymmetry, emotion, and affective style. In R.J. Davidson & K. Hugdahl (Eds.), Brain Asymmetry pp. 361–387. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J., & Hermans, D. (1994). Differences in the affective processing of words and pictures. Cognition and Emotion, 8, 1–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dijksterhuis, A. & Aarts, H. (2003). On wildebeests and humans: The preferential detection of negative stimuli. Psychological Science, 14 (1), 14–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, N.F. (1990). Perceptual and other related defenses. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 21 (4), 319–330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Draine, S.C., & Greenwald, A.G. (1998). Replicable unconscious semantic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127(3), 286–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, N., & Lennon, R. (1983). Sex differences in empathy and related capacities. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 100–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eviatar, Z., & Zaidel, E. (1991). The effects of word length and emotionality on hemispheric contribution to lexical decision. Neuropsychologia, 29 (5), 415–428.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R.H. (2001). On the automatic activation of associated evaluations: An overview. Cognition and Emotion, 15 (2), 115–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R.H., Roskos-Ewoldsen, D.R., & Powell, M.C. (1994) Attitudes, perception, and attention. In Niedenthal, P. M., & Kitayama, S. (Eds.) The Heart's Eye: Emotional Influences in Perception and Attention. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R.H., Sanbonmatsu, D.M., Powell, M.C., & Kardes, F.R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 229–238.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fiedler, K. (2001). Affective influences on social information processing. In Forgas, J.P. (Ed.) Handbook of Affect and Social Cognition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, D.M., & Swets, J.A. (1966). Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A.G., Draine, S.C., & Abrams, R. L. (1996). Three cognitive markers of unconscious semantic activation. Science, 273, (5282), 1699–1702.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grunwald, I. S., Borod, J.C., Obler, L.K., Erhan, H.M., & Pick, L.H. (1999). The effects of age and gender on the perception of lexical emotion. Applied Neuropsychology, 6 (4), 226–238.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J.A. (1978). Gender effects in decoding nonverbal cues. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 845–875.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hansen, C.H. & Hansen, R.D. (1994). Automatic emotion: Attention and facial efference. In Niedenthal, P. M. & Kitayama, S. (Eds.) The Heart's Eye: Emotional Influences in Perception and Attention. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heller, W., Nitsche, J.B., & Miller, G.A. (1998). Lateralization in emotion and emotional disorders. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7(1), 26–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heilige, J. B. (2001). Hemispheric Asymmetry: What's right and what's left. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, D., De Houwer, J., & Eelen, P. (1994). The affective priming effect: Automatic activation of evaluative information in memory. Cognition and Emotion, 8, 515–533.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. (1984). Toward an understanding of the role of affect in cognition. In R. Wyer & T. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A.M. (1985). The asymmetry of happiness and sadness in effects on memory in normal college students. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 388–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A.M. (1993) Positive affect and decision making. In Lewis, M. & Haviland, J. M. (Eds.). Handbook of Emotions. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ito, T.A. & Cacioppo, J.T. (2001). Affect and attitudes: A social neuroscience approach. In Forgas, J. P. (Ed.). Handbook of Affect and Social Cognition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ito, T.A., Larsen, J.T., Smith, N.K., & Cacioppo, J.T. (1998). Negative information weighs more heavily on the brain: The negativity bias in evaluative categorizations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74 (4), 887–900.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitayama, S., & Howard, S. (1994) Affective regulation of perception and comprehension: Amplification and semantic priming. In Niedenthal, P.M. & Kitayama, S. (Eds.) The Heart's Eye: Emotional Influences in Perception and Attention. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, J.T, McGraw, A.P., & Cacioppo, J.T. (2001). Can people feel happy and sad at the same time? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 684–696.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ladavas, E., Umilta, C., & Ricci-Bitti, P.E. (1980). Evidence for sex differences in right-hemisphere dominance for emotions. Neuropsychologica, 18, (3), 361–366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J.E. (1993). Emotional networks in the brain. In Lewis, M. & Haviland, J. M. (Eds.) Handbook of Emotions. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyers, M.B., & Smith, B.D. (1987). Cerebral processing of nonverbal affective stimuli: Differential effects of cognitive and affective sets on hemispheric asymmetry. Biological Psychology, 24, (1), 67–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Obrzut, J.E., Bryden, M.P., Lang, P, & Bulman-Fleming, M.B. (2001). Concurrent verbal and emotion laterality effects exhibited by normally achieving and learning disabled children. Child Neuropsychology, 7(3), 153–161.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ohira, H., Winton, W.M., & Oyama, M. (1998). Effects of stimulus valence on recognition memory and endogenous eyeblinks: Further evidence for positive-negative asymmetry. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24 (9), 986–993.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pratto, F. (1994). Consciousness and automatic evaluation. In Niedenthal, P.M. & Kitayama, S. (Eds.) The Heart's Eye: Emotional Influences in Perception and Attention. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pratto, F. & John, O.P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: The attention grabbing power of negative social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41 (3), 380–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R., & DePaulo, B.M. (1979). Sex differences in eavesdropping on nonverbal cues. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 273–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P. & Royzman, E.B. (2001). Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, (4), 296–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Segerstrom, S.C. (2001). Optimism and attentional bias for positive and negative stimuli. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(10), 1334–1343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silberman, E.K., & Weingartner, H. (1968). Hemispheric lateralization of functions related to emotion. Brain and Cognition, 5, 322–353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, N.K. (2002). Validating and eliminating the attentional bias to negative information. Dissertation Abstracts International, March, 62 (8-B).

  • Smith, N.K., Cacioppo, J.T., Larsen, J.T., & Chartrand, T.L. (2003). May I have your attention, please: Electrocortical responses to positive and negative stmuli. Neuropsychologia, 41 (2), 171–183.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snodgrass, L., & Harring, K. (1992, June). Subliminal perception evaluated in a signal detection paradigm. Poster presented at the International Congress of Psychology, Brussels, Belgium.

  • Taylor, S.E. (1991). Asyymetrical effects of positive and negative events: The mobilization-minimization hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin., 110, (1), 67–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Strien, J.W. & Morpurgo, M. (1992). Opposite hemispheric activations as a result of emotionally threatening and non-threatening words. Neuropsychologia, 30, (9), 845–848.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White, M. (1996). Automatic affective appraisal of words. Cognition and Emotion., 10 (2), 199–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wild, B., Erb, M., & Bartels, M. (2001). Are emotions contagious? Evoked emotions while viewing emotionally expressive faces: Quality, quantity, time course, and gender differences. Psychiatry Research, 102, (2), 109–124.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yahnke, B. K. (1988-89). The effects of functional brain asymmetry upon subliminal perception. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, 8, 121–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R.B., Pietromonaco, P., & Bargh, J. (1982). Independence and interaction of affect and cognition. In M.S. Clark & S.T. Fiske (Eds.), Affect and cognition: The seventeenth annual Carnegie symposium on cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Snodgrass, L.L., Harring, K.E. Right Hemisphere Positivity Bias in Preconscious Processing: Data From Five Experiments. Curr Psychol 23, 318–335 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-004-1004-3

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-004-1004-3

Keywords

Navigation