Skip to main content
Log in

Age differences in memory for emotional messages: Do older people always remember the positive?

  • Research Papers
  • Invited Paper
  • Published:
Ageing International Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Theories and empirical research in the Western literature suggest that with age, people are more motivated to derive emotion meaning from life, and one way to do so is to disproportionably remember the positive in life. We examined this bias—known as the positivity bias—among younger (aged 18–27 years; n=91) and older (aged 52–85 years, n=94) Hong Kong Chinese, by presenting them with health-promoting messages that had the same content but different background music. We found that, in contrast to prior findings obtained in the West, older Hong Kong Chinese remembered the most information from messages with negative, relative to positive or neutral, background music. Younger adults did not show these biases.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Balkwill, L.L., & Thompson, W.F. (1999). A cross-cultural investigation of the perception of emotion in music: Psychophysical and cultural cues. Music Perception, 17(1), 43–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G.H. (1992). How might emotions affect learning? In S.-A. Christianson (Ed.), The handbook of emotion and memory: Research and theory (pp. 3–31). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carstensen, L.L. (1995). Evidence for a life-span theory of socioemotional selectivity. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4, 151–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carstensen, L.L. (1992). Social and emotional patterns in adulthood: Support for socioemotional selectivity theory. Psychology and Aging, 7, 331–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carstensen, L.L., Fung, H.H., & Charles, S.T. (2003). Socioemotional selectivity theory and the regulation of emotion in the second half of life. Motivation and Emotion, 27, 103–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carstensen, L.L., Isaacowitz, D., & Charles, S.T. (1999). Taking time seriously: a theory of socioemotional selectivity. American Psychologist, 54, 165–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carstensen, L.L., & Turk-Charles, S. (1994). The salience of emotion across the adult life span. Psychology and Aging, 9, 259–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carstensen, L.L., Pasupathi, M., Mayr, U., & Nesselroade, J.R. (2000). Emotional experience in everyday life across the adult life span. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(4), 644–655.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charles, S., Mather, M., & Carstensen, L.L. (2003). Aging and emotional memory: The forgettable nature of negative images for older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132(2), 310–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Derryberry, D. (1989). Effects of goal-related motivational states on the spatial orienting of attention. Acta Psychologica, 72, 199–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., & Suh, E. (1997). Measuring quality of life: Economic, social, and subjective indicators. Social Indicators Research, 40(1–2), 189–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Falbo, T., Poston, D.L., Jr., Triscari, S., & Zhang, X. (1997). Self-enhancing illusions among Chinese school children. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology, 28, 172–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fleming, J.S., & Courtney, B.E. (1984). The dimensionality of self-esteem: II. Hierarchical facet model for revised measurements scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 25–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frey, D., & Stahlberg, D. (1986). Selection of information after receiving more or less reliable self-threatening information. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 12, 434–441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fung, H.H., & Carstensen, L.L. (2003). Sending memorable messages to the old: Age differences in preferences and memory for advertisements. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 163–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fung, H.H., Carstensen, L.L., & Lutz, M.A. (1999). Influence of time on social preferences: Implications for life-span development. Psychology and Aging, 14, 595–604.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fung, H.H., Lai, P., & Ng, R. (2001). Age differences in social preferences among Taiwanese and Mainland Chinese: The role of perceived time. Psychology and Aging, 16, 351–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gould, O.N., & Dixon, R.A. (1993). How we spent our vacation: Collaborative storytelling by young and old adults. Psychology and Aging, 8, 10–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, J.J., Carstensen, L.C., Pasupathi, M., Tsai, J., Gotestam-Skorpen, K., & Hsu, A.Y.C. (1997). Emotion and aging: Experience, expression, and control. Psychology and Aging, 12, 590–599.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hashtroudi, S., Johnson, M.K., & Chroniak, L.D. (1990). Aging and qualitative characteristics of memories for perceived and imagined complex events. Psychology and Aging, 5, 119–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herzog, A.R., Franks, M.M., Markus, H.R., & Holmberg, D. (1998). Activities and well-being in older age: Effects of self-concept and educational attainment. Psychology and Aging, 13, 179–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isaacowitz, D.M., Wadlinger, H.A., Goren, D., & Wilson, H.R. (in press). Selective bias in visual fixation to negative images in old age? An eye tracking study. Psychology and Aging.

  • Johnson, M.K., & Sherman, S.J. (1990). Constructing and reconstructing the past and the future in the present. In Higgins, E.T., and Sorrentino, R.M. (Eds). Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior, Vol. 2, (pp. 482–526). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keller, M.L., Leventhal, H., Prohaska, T.R., & Leventhal, E.A. (1989). Beliefs about aging and illness in a community sample. Research in Nursing and Health, 12, 247–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy, Q., Mather, M., & Carstensen, L.L. (2004) The role of motivation in the age-related positive bias in autobiographical memory. Psychological Science, 15(3), 208–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitayama, S., & Karasawa, M. (1995). Self: A cultural psychological perspective. Japanese Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 133–163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitayama, S., Markus, H.R., Matsumoto, H., & Norasakkunkit, V. (1997). Individual and collective processes in the construction of the self: Self-enhancement in the United States and self-criticism in Japan. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1245–1267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, F.R., & Carstensen, L.L. (2002). Time counts: Future time perspective, goals, and social relationships. Psychology and Aging, 17, 125–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levine, L.J., & Bluck, S. (1997). Experienced and remembered emotional intensity in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 12, 514–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H.R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mather, M., Canli, T., English, T., Whitfield, S., Wais, P., Ochsner, K., Gabrieli, J.D.E., & Carstensen, L.L. (2004). Amygdala responses to emotionally valenced stimuli in older and younger adults. Psychological Science, 15, 259–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mather, M., & Carstensen, L.L. (2003). Aging and attentional biases for emotional faces. Psychological Science, 14, 409–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mather, M., & Johnson, M.K. (2000). Choice-supportive source monitoring: Do our decisions seem better to us as we age? Psychology & Aging, 15, 596–606.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mroczek, D.K., & Kolarz, C.M. (1998). The effect of age on positive and negative affect: A developmental perspective on happiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1333–1349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ostwald, P.F. (1966). Music and human emotions: Discussion. Journal of Music Therapy, 3(3), 93–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oyserman, D., Coon, H.M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 3–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Park, D.C., Nisbett, R., & Hedden, T. (1999). Aging, culture, and cognition. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 54B, P75-P84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peng, K., & Nisbett, R.E. (1999). Dialectical responses to questions about dialectical thinking. American Psychologist, 55, 1067–1068.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R.E., & Cacioppo, J.T. (1984). The effects of involvement on response to argument quantity and argument quality: Central and peripheral routes to persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 69–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quackenbush, S.W., & Barnett, M.A. (2001). Recollection and evaluation of critical experiences in moral development: A cross-sectional examination. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 23(1), 55–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robazza, C., Macaluso, C., & D’urso, V. (1994). Emotional reactions to music by gender, age, and expertise. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 79, 934–944.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothman, A.J., & Salovey, P. (1997). Shaping perceptions to motivate healthy behavior: The role of message framing. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 3–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schulkind, M.D., Hennis, L.K., & Rubin, D.C. (1999). Music, emotion, and autobiographic memory: They’re playing your song. Memory and Cognition, 27(6), 948–955.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silber, E., & Tippett, J.S. (1965). Self-esteem: Clinical assessment and measurement validation. Psychological Reports, 16, 1017–1071.

    Google Scholar 

  • Triandis, H. (1989). The self and social behavior in differing cultural contexts. Psychological Review, 96, 506–520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Clark, L., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1981). The psychometric tradition: Developing the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. Contemporary Education Psychology, 6, 82–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Helene H. Fung.

Additional information

Her research interests lie in social motivation and cognition across adulthood. She obtained her Ph.D from Stanford University. She is the winner of the 1999 Behavioral and Social Science Pre-dissertation Research Award, offered by Gerontological Society of America, and the 1998 Margaret Clark Paper Award, offered by the Association of Anthropology and Gerontology. She is an associate editor of the Journal of Psychology in Chinese Societies.

Lilian Y.T. Tang obtained her undergraduate degree in psychology from the Chinese University of Hong Kong.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Fung, H.H., Tang, L.Y.T. Age differences in memory for emotional messages: Do older people always remember the positive?. Ageing Int. 30, 245–262 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12126-005-1014-y

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12126-005-1014-y

Keywords

Navigation