Skip to main content
Log in

The Machine Paradigm and Alternative Approaches in Cognitive Science

  • Published:
Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In a recent paper called To think human out of the machine paradigm, it is stated that psychological science operates within a machine paradigm that is committed to mechanical causality. In addition, it is emphasizes the epistemological and methodological limitations of explanations based in deterministic mechanics and instead argues for the need of an ‘organic paradigm’ that takes into consideration psychological processes such as subjectivity, inter-subjectivity, and agency. Although there is no doubt that much psychological science has operated under a machine paradigm, we argue that recent psychological research is pursued using a wide variety of approaches and with an absence of a partially integrated meta-theoretical corpus. The present situation looks more like a Tower of Babel of epistemological approaches and empirical programs. The reconsideration of the organic paradigm and an explicitly addressed epistemological framework could constitute a step forward and lead to an explanatory pluralism built on greater dialogue within the psychological sciences.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anderson, M. (2003). Embodied cognition: a field guide. Artificial Intelligence, 149, 91–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bakhurst, D. (1995). Lessons from Ilyenkov. The Communication Review, 1, 155–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barutta, J., Gleichgerrcht, E., Cornejo, C., & Ibáñez, A. (2010). Neurodynamics of mind: the arrow illusion of conscious intentionality as downward causation. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science.

  • Bechtel, W. (1998). Representations and cognitive explanations: assessing the dynamicist challenge in cognitive science. Cognitive Science, 22, 295–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bechtel, W., & Richardson, R. C. (1993). Discovering complexity: Decomposition and localization as strategies in scientific research. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bechtel, W., & Graham, G. (Eds.). (1998). A companion to cognitive science. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bechtel, W., Mandik, P., Mundale, J., & Stufflebeam, R. (Eds.). (2001). Philosophy and the neurosciences: A reader. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bem, S. (2001). The explanatory autonomy of psychology: why a mind is not a brain. Theory & Psychology, 11, 785–795.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bickhard, M. (1999). Interaction and representation. Theory & Psychology, 9(4), 435–458.

  • Bickhard, M. H. (2003). Frontiers of Interactivism. Interactivist Summer Institute 2003. Copenhagen, Denmark, 26 July 2003.

  • Bickhard, M. H. (2004). Process and emergence: normative function and representation. International journal in ontology and cognitive systems, 14, 135–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloor, D. (1996). Idealism and the sociology of knowledge. Social Studies of Science, 26, 839–856.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brighton, H., Smith, K., & Kirby, S. (2003). Situated cognition and the role of multi-agent models in explaining language structure. In D. Kudenko, E. Alonso, & D. Kazakov (Eds.), Adaptive agents and multi-agents systems: Adaptation and multi-agent learning. NY: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bromberger, S. (1966). Why-Questions. In R. G. Colodny (Ed.), Mind and cosmos: Essays in contemporary science and philosophy (1st ed., Vol. 3, pp. 299–358). Pittsburg: The Center for Philosophy of Science, University of Pittsburgh.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chemero, A. (2001). Dynamical explanations and mental representations. Trends in Cognitive Science, 5, 141–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chemero, A. (2003). Radical empiricism through the ages. Contemporary Psychology, 34, 456–478.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cilliers, P. (1998). Complexity and postmodernism: Understanding complex systems. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (1997). Being there: Putting brain, body and world together again. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2001). Reasons, robots and the extended mind. Mind and Language, 16, 121–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A., & Chalmers, D. (1998). The extended mind. Analysis, 58, 10–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M., & Engeströn, Y. (1991). A cultural historical approach to distributed cognition. In G. Salomon (Ed.), Distributed cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins, H. M. (1998). The meaning of data: open and closed evidential cultures in the search for gravitational waves. American Journal of Sociology, 104, 293–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cornejo, C., Simonetti, F., Aldunate, N., Ibáñez, A., López, V., & Melloni, L. (2007). Electrophysiological evidence of different interpretive strategies in irony comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research. doi:10.1007/s10936-007-9052-0.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cornejo, C., Simonetti, F., Ibañez, A., Aldunate, N., Lopez, V., Ceric, F., et al. (2009). Gesture and metaphor: electrophysiological evidence of N400 multimodal modulation. Brain and Cognition. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2008.12.005.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cosmelli, D., & Ibañez, A. (2008). Human cognition in context: on the biologic, cognitive and social reconsideration of meaning as making sense of action. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Sciences, 42, 233–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Craver, C. (2000). Role functions. Mechanisms, and Hierarchy, Philosophy of Science, 68, 53–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Jong, L. (2001). A symposium on explanatory pluralism. Theory & Psychology, 11, 860–863.

    Google Scholar 

  • Descombes, V. (2001). The mind’s provisions: A critique of cognitivism. Hardcover: Princeton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dietrich, E. (2000). Cognitive Science and the Mechanistic Forces of Darkness, or Why the Computational Science of Mind Suffers the Slings and Arrows of Outrageous Fortune. Techné: eJournal of the Society for Philosophy and Technology, Winter.

  • Diriwächter, R. (2004). Ganzheitspsychologie: the doctrine. From Past to Future, 5(1), 3–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dreyfus, H., & Dreyfus, S. (1990). Making a mind versus modelling the brain: Artificial intelligence back at a branch-point. In M. Boden (Ed.), The philosophy of artificial intelligence (pp. 308–333). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dufey, M., Hurtado, E., Fernández, A. M., & Ibáñez. (2010, In press). Exploring the relationship between vagal tone and event-related potentials in response to an affective picture task. Social Neuroscience.

  • Emmeche, C., Koppe, S., & Stjernfelt, F. (2000). Levels, emergence, and three versions of downward causation. In P. Bogh, C. Emmeche, N. Finneman, & P. Christiansen (Eds.), Downward causation (pp. 13–34). Aarhus: Aarhus University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engelsted, N. (1993). At a crossroads. An introduction. In N. Engelsted et al. (Eds.), The societal subject. Aaurus: Aarhus University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feyerabend, P. (1978). Against method. London: Verso.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, W. (2003). Mathematics, measurement, metaphor and metaphysics II: accounting for Galileo’s ‘fateful omission’. Theory Psychology, 13(6), 791–828.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, W., & Nuñez, R. (1999). Restoring to cognition the forgotten primacy of action, intention and emotion. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6, 3–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, E. (Ed.). (2000). Integrative neuroscience. London: Haword Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, E. (2003). Integrative neuroscience. Neuropsychofarmacology, 28, S2–S8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haken, H. (1997). Visions of synergetics. International Journal of Bifurcation and Chaos, 7(9), 1927–1951.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haken, H. (2003). Intelligent behavior - A synergetic view. In W. Tschacher & J-P. Dauwalder (Eds.) Dynamical Systems Approaches to Embodied Cognition. Singapore: World Scientic.

  • Haken, H., Kelso, J., & Bunz, H. (1985). A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements. Biological Cybernetics, 51, 347–356.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hanson, N. R. (1958). Patterns of discovery. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haugeland, J. (1995). Having thought: Essay in the metaphysics of mind. Harvard: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, L., Reese, H. W., & Sarbin, T. (Eds.). (1993). Varieties of scientific contextualism. Reno: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoshmand, L. (2003). Can lessons of history and logical analysis ensure progress in psychological science? Theory & Psychology, 13(1), 39–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hurtado, E, Gonzalez, R., Haye, A; Manes, F., & Ibanez, A. (2009). Contextual blending of ingroup/outgroup face stimuli and word valence: LPP modulation and convergence of measures. BMC Neuroscience.

  • Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ibáñez, A. (2007a). Complexity and cognition: mind and brain as a topological dynamical system. Nonlinear Dynamics, Psychology, and Life Sciences, 11(1), 51–90.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ibáñez, A. (2007b). The dynamic core of consciousness and neural Darwinism. Journal of Neurology, 1–15;45(9):547–555.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ibañez, A. (2008). Dinámica de la Cognición [Dynamics of Cognition]. JCSaez: Chile.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ibañez, A., & Cosmelli, D. (2008). Moving beyond computational cognitivism: understanding intentionality. Intersubjectivity and Ecology of Mind Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Sciences, 42(2), 129–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ibáñez, A., Lopez, V., & Cornejo, C. (2006). ERPs and contextual semantic discrimination: evidence of degrees of congruency in wakefulness and sleep. Brain and Language, 98(3), 264–275.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ibáñez, A., Haye, A., González, R., Hurtado, E., & Henríquez, R. (2009). Multi-level analysis of cultural phenomena: the role of ERP approach to prejudice. The Journal for Theory in Social Behavior, 39, 81–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ibáñez, A., Manes, F., Escobar, J., Trujillo, N., Andreucci, P., & Hurtado, E. (2010). Gesture influences the processing of figurative language in non-native speakers. Neuroscience Letters, 471(2010), 48–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jencks, C. (1987). What is post-modernism? London: Academy Editions.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. (1987). The body in the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohler, A. (2010). Think human out of the machine paradigm. Homo Ex Machina. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 44(1), 39–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kusch, M. (Ed.). (2000). The sociology of philosophical knowledge. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakatos, I. (1974). Falsification and the methodology of scientific research programs. In I. Lakatos & A. Musgrave (Eds.), Criticism and the growth of knowledge (pp. 91–195). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (1987). Science in action: How to follow scientists and engineers through society. Cambridge: Harvard UP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laudan, L. (1977). Progress and its problems. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lighthill, M. J. (1973). Artificial intelligence: A general survey in artificial intelligence: a paper symposium. UK: Science Research Council.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyotard, J.-F. (1991). The postmodern condition: A report on knowledge. Manchester: University of Manchester Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maull, N. (1977). Unifying science without reduction. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science, 8, 143–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mikulecky, D. (1999). Robert Rosen. USA: Virginia University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundale, J., & Bechtel, W. (1996). Integrating neuroscience, psychology, and evolutionary biology through a teleological view of function. Minds and Machines, 6, 481–505.

    Google Scholar 

  • Popper, K. (1959). Logic of scientific discovery. London: Hutchinson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reese, H. W. (2001). Review of Capaldi and Proctor’s contextualism in psychological research? A critical review. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, 379–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riveros, R., Manes, H., Escobar, E., Reyes, M. C., & Ibañez, M. (2010). Context-sensitive social cognition is impaired in schizophrenic patients and their healthy relatives. Schizophrenia Research, 116(2010), 297–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E., & Zipser, D. (1986). Feature discovery by competitive learning. In Parallel Distributed Processing, (pp. 151–193). Cambridge: MIT Press.

  • Ryle, G. (1949). The concept of mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salmon, W. (1989). Four decades of scientific explanation. In P. Kitcher & W. Salmon (Eds.), Minnesota studies in the philosophy of science, Vol. 13 (1st ed., pp. 3–219). Minnesota: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • San Martín, R., Manes, F., Hurtado, E., Isla, P., Ibáñez, A. (2010). Size and probability of rewards modulate the feedback error-related negativity associated with wins but not losses in a monetarily rewarded gambling task. NeuroImage. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.031.

  • Schouten, M. K., & de Jong, L. (1996). Over functies en affordances. Algemeen Nederlands Tijdschrift voor Wijsbegeerte, 88, 216–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scriven, M. (1962). Explanations, predictions, and laws. In H. Feigl & G. Maxwell (Eds.), Scientific explanation, space, and time (1st ed., Vol. 3, pp. 170–230). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shook, J., & Ghiraldelli, P. (2004). Contemporary pragmatism. NY: Rodopi.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanley, J. (2004). On the linguistic basis for contextualism. Philosophical Studies, 119, 23–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stegmüller, W. (1976). The structure and dynamics of theories. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, E., & Varela, F. (2001). Radical embodiment: neural dynamics and consciouness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 418–425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tschacher, W., & Dauwalder, J. P. (Eds.). (2003). The dynamical systems approach to cognition. Singapore: World Scientific.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Fraassen, B. (1980). The scientific image. Oxford: The Clarendon Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Van Fraassen, B. (2000a). The false hopes of traditional epistemology. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 40, 253–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Fraassen, B. (2000b). Constructive empiricism now. Philosophical Studies, 106, 151–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F., Thompsom, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler, M. (1996). From Robots to Rothko. In M. Boden (Ed.), The philosophy of artificial life. Amsterdam: Dress Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1952). Philosophical investigations. Oxford: Blackwell.

  • Wolfram, S. (2002). A new kind of science. NY: Wolfram press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, J., Rennie, C., Lees, G., Robinson, P., Bourke, P., Chapman, C., et al. (2003). Simulated electrocortical activity at microscopic, mesoscopic and global scales. Neuropsychopharmacology, 28, 80–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Agustín Ibáñez.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Barutta, J., Aravena, P. & Ibáñez, A. The Machine Paradigm and Alternative Approaches in Cognitive Science. Integr. psych. behav. 44, 176–183 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-010-9116-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-010-9116-9

Keywords

Navigation