Abstract
Dehumanization is observed in adults across cultures and is thought to motivate human violence. The age of its first expression remains largely untested. This research demonstrates that diverse representations of humanness, including a novel one, readily elicit blatant dehumanization in adults (N = 482) and children (aged 5–12; N = 150). Dehumanizing responses in both age groups are associated with support for outgroup inferiority. Similar to the link previously observed in adults, dehumanization by children is associated with a willingness to punish outgroup transgressors. These findings suggest that exposure to cultural norms throughout adolescence and adulthood are not required for the development of outgroup dehumanization.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Bain, P., Vaes, J., Kashima, Y., Haslam, N., & Guan, Y. (2012). Folk conceptions of humanness: Beliefs about distinctive and core human characteristics in Australia, Italy, and China. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 43(1), 53–58
Bandura, A. (2016). Moral disengagement: How people do harm and live with themselves. Worth, Macmillan
Bandura, A., Underwood, B., & Fromson, M. E. (1975). Disinhibition of aggression through diffusion of responsibility and dehumanization of victims. Journal of Research in Personality, 9(4), 253–269
Bloom, P. (2017). The root of all cruelty? The New Yorker. https://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2017/11/27/the-root-of-all-cruelty
Bohman, A., & Miklikowska, M. (2021). Does classroom diversity improve intergroup relations? Short-and long-term effects of classroom diversity for cross-ethnic friendships and anti-immigrant attitudes in adolescence. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 24(8), 1372–1390
Bruneau, E., Jacoby, N., Kteily, N., & Saxe, R. (2018). Denying humanity: The distinct neural correlates of blatant dehumanization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(7), 1078–1093
Bruneau, E., Szekeres, H., Kteily, N., Tropp, L. R., & Kende, A. (2020). Beyond dislike: Blatant dehumanization predicts teacher discrimination. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 23(4), 560–577
Chas, A., Betancor, V., Delgado, N., & Rodríguez-Pérez, A. (2018). Children consider their own group to be more human than other social groups: Evidence from indirect and direct measures. Social Psychology, 49(3), 125
Cikara, M., Bruneau, E. G., & Saxe, R. R. (2011). Us and them: Intergroup failures of empathy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(3), 149–153
Cikara, M., Eberhardt, J. L., & Fiske, S. T. (2011). From agents to objects: Sexist attitudes and neural responses to sexualized targets. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(3), 540–551
Cloutier, J., Ambady, N., Meagher, T., & Gabrieli, J. (2012). The neural substrates of person perception: Spontaneous use of financial and moral status knowledge. Neuropsychologia, 50(9), 2371–2376
Costello, K., & Hodson, G. (2011). Social dominance-based threat reactions to immigrants in need of assistance. European Journal of Social Psychology, 41(2), 220–231
Costello, K., & Hodson, G. (2014). Explaining dehumanization among children: The interspecies model of prejudice. British Journal of Social Psychology, 53(1), 175–197
Dhont, K., Hodson, G., Loughnan, S., & Amiot, C. E. (2019). Rethinking human-animal relations: The critical role of social psychology. Group Process & Intergroup Relations, 22(6), 769–784
Dore, R. A., Hoffman, K. M., Lillard, A. S., & Trawalter, S. (2014). Children’s racial bias in perceptions of others’ pain. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 32(2), 218–231
Farrow, T. F., Jones, S. C., Kaylor-Hughes, C. J., Wilkinson, I. D., Woodruff, P. W., Hunter, M. D., & Spence, S. A. (2011). Higher or lower? The functional anatomy of perceived allocentric social hierarchies. Neuroimage, 57(4), 1552–1560
Fritz, C. O., Morris, P. E., & Richler, J. J. (2012). Effect size estimates: current use, calculations, and interpretation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(1), 2–18
Goff, P. A., Eberhardt, J. L., Williams, M. J., & Jackson, M. C. (2008). Not yet human: implicit knowledge, historical dehumanization, and contemporary consequences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(2), 292–306
Hare, B. (2001). Can competitive paradigms increase the validity of experiments on primate social cognition? Animal Cognition, 4(3), 269–280
Hare, B. (2017). Survival of the friendliest: Homo sapiens evolved via selection for prosociality. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 155–186
Hare, B., & Woods, V. (2021). Survival of the friendliest: Understanding our origins and rediscovering our common humanity. Random House
Harris, L. T. (2017). Invisible mind: Flexible social cognition and dehumanization. MIT Press
Harris, L. T., & Fiske, S. T. (2006). Dehumanizing the lowest of the low: Neuroimaging responses to extreme out-groups. Psychological Science, 17(10), 847–853
Haslam, N., & Loughman, S. (2014). Dehumanization and infrahumanization. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 399–423. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115045
Ho, A. K., Sidanius, J., Kteily, N., Sheehy-Skeffington, J., Pratto, F., Henkel, K. E. … Stewart, A. L. (2015). The nature of social dominance orientation: Theorizing and measuring preferences for intergroup inequality using the new SDO7 scale. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(6), 1003–1028
Hodson, G., Crisp, R. J., Meleady, R., & Earle, M. (2018). Intergroup contact as an agent of cognitive liberalization. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(5), 523–548
House, B. R., Silk, J. B., Henrich, J., Barrett, H. C., Scelza, B. A., Boyette, A. H. … Laurence, S. (2013). Ontogeny of prosocial behavior across diverse societies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(36), 14586–14591
Jardina, A., & Piston, S. (2021). The effects of dehumanizing attitudes about Black people on Whites’ voting decisions. British Journal of Political Science, 1–23. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123421000089
Kteily, N., & Bruneau, E. (2017). Darker demons of our nature: The need to (re) focus attention on blatant forms of dehumanization. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(6), 487–494
Kteily, N., Bruneau, E., Waytz, A., & Cotterill, S. (2015). The ascent of man: Theoretical and empirical evidence for blatant dehumanization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(5), 901–931. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000048
Kteily, N., Hodson, G., & Bruneau, E. (2016). They see us as less than human: Metadehumanization predicts intergroup conflict via reciprocal dehumanization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110(3), 343
Levy, J., Goldstein, A., Influs, M., Masalha, S., Zagoory-Sharon, O., & Feldman, R. (2016). Adolescents growing up amidst intractable conflict attenuate brain response to pain of outgroup. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(48), 13696–13701
Markowitz, D. M., & Slovic, P. (2020). Social, psychological, and demographic characteristics of dehumanization toward immigrants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 117(17), 9260–9269
Martin, J., Bennett, M., & Murray, W. S. (2008). A developmental study of the infrahumanization hypothesis. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 26(2), 153–162
McCallum, C. (2001). Gender and sociality in Amazonia: How real people are made. Berg
McLoughlin, N., Tipper, S. P., & Over, H. (2018). Young children perceive less humanness in outgroup faces. Developmental Science, 21(2), e12539
Muthukrishna, M., Bell, A. V., Henrich, J., Curtin, C. M., Gedranovich, A., McInerney, J., & Thue, B. (2020). Beyond western, educated, industrial, rich, and democratic (WEIRD) psychology: measuring and mapping scales of cultural and psychological distance. Psychological Science, 31(6), 678–701
Over, H. (2021). Seven challenges for the dehumanization hypothesis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(1), 3–13
Richins, M. T., Barreto, M., Karl, A., & Lawrence, N. (2019). Empathic responses are reduced to competitive but not non-competitive outgroups. Social neuroscience, 14(3), 345–358
Rodríguez, V. B., Villar, A. C., Rodríguez-Pérez, A., & Rodríguez, N. D. (2016). Infrahumanization in children: An evaluation of 70 terms relating to humanity. Psicothema, 28(1), 53–58
Smith, D. L. (2020). On inhumanity: Dehumanization and how to resist it. Oxford University Press
Sumner, W. G. (1906). Folkways: A study of the sociological importance of usages, manners, customs, mores, and morals. Ginn
Tomasello, M. (2019). Becoming human: A theory of ontogeny. Harvard University Press
Trounson, J. S., Critchley, C., & Pfeifer, J. E. (2015). Australian attitudes toward asylum seekers: Roles of dehumanization and social dominance theory. Social Behavior and Personality, 43(10), 1641–1655
van Noorden, T. H., Haselager, G. J., Cillessen, A. H., & Bukowski, W. M. (2014). Dehumanization in children: The link with moral disengagement in bullying and victimization. Aggressive Behavior, 40(4), 320–328
Vezzali, L., Hewstone, M., Capozza, D., Trifiletti, E., & Bernardo, G. A. D. (2017). Improving intergroup relations with extended contact among young children: Mediation by intergroup empathy and moderation by direct intergroup contact. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 27(1), 35–49
Viki, G. T., Osgood, D., & Phillips, S. (2013). Dehumanization and self-reported proclivity to torture prisoners of war. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(3), 325–328
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory‐of‐mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655–684
Wilks, M., Caviola, L., Kahane, G., & Bloom, P. (2021). Children prioritize humans over animals less than adults do. Psychological Science, 32(1), 27–38
Wrangham, R. (2019). The goodness paradox: The strange relationship between virtue and violence in human evolution. Pantheon
Acknowledgements
We thank Zoo Atlanta for hosting our research, to the parents and children for participating and to Aleah Bowie, Erica Blair, Feruth Kidane, Julia Villegas, and Morgan Ferrans who helped collect the data. We thank Philip White and Zachary White who gave us advice on statistics. We also wish to thank Aleah Bowie, Michael Tomasello, Sarah Gaither and Ashley Jardina for invaluable comments they provided on an earlier version of the manuscript. Funding for this work was provided by Duke University.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Both authors contributed to the study design. W.Z. performed testing, data collection, data analysis and interpretation under the supervision of B.H. Both authors wrote the manuscript and approved the final version of the manuscript for submission.
Corresponding author
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Electronic Supplementary Material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Zhou, W., Hare, B. The Early Expression of Blatant Dehumanization in Children and Its Association with Outgroup Negativity. Hum Nat 33, 196–214 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-022-09427-x
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-022-09427-x