Skip to main content
Log in

A Naturalistic Study of Norm Conformity, Punishment, and the Veneration of the Dead at Texas A&M University, USA

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Culturally inherited institutional norms structure much of human social life. Successfully replicating institutions train their current members to behave in the generally adaptive ways that served past members. Ancestor veneration is a well-known manifestation of this phenomenon whereby deference is conferred to prestigious past members who are used as cultural models. Such norms of respect may be maintained by punishment based on evidence from theory and laboratory experiments, but there is little observational evidence to show that punishment is commonly used. To test for punishment as a mechanism that maintains these norms, we examine a norm of ancestor veneration in a natural field experiment at the Memorial Student Center (MSC) at Texas A&M University. The MSC is a memorial to university war dead, and the expectation is that all who enter the building remove their hats out of respect. Observations reveal that hat removal is significantly more common at the MSC than at two control locations. Survey data indicate that most, but not all, subjects understand the norm to be veneration of the dead, and most expect others to follow the norm. Many report a strong negative emotional response when asked to imagine the norm being violated. Sixty-two percent report they would definitely or probably ask the noncompliant to uncover. Focal follow data show that punishment is relatively rare, however, with the majority of behatted subjects going unreproached as they pass through the building. Both survey and observational data indicate there is a motivated minority that enthusiastically enforces the norm.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Data Availability

Available on request.

Code availability

N/A

Notes

  1. Those observed in a Corps of Cadets uniform were removed from analysis. Since corps members only wear their uniforms on campus, these individuals were only seen at Evans Library and the MSC and they always removed their hats.

  2. The term “t-sip” is a pejorative directed at students from Texas A&M’s archrival, the University of Texas (UT). “t.u.” is a derogatory way of referring to UT.

References

  • Alesina, A., & Giuliano, P. (2015). Culture and institutions. Journal of Economic Literature, 53(4), 898–944.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Altmann, J. (1974). Observational study of behavior: Sampling methods. Behaviour, 49(3–4), 227–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alvard, M. (2003). The adaptive nature of culture. Evolutionary Anthropology, 12(3), 136–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alvard, M., & Nolin, D. (2002). Rousseau’s whale hunt? Coordination among big-game hunters. Current Anthropology, 43(4), 533–559.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anonymous. (2017). Corps of cadets uniform wear policy. Retrieved from: https://cadets.tamu.edu/wp-content/uploads/Uniform-Policy.pdf. Accessed 30 Nov 2018.

  • Axelrod, R. (1986). An evolutionary approach to norms. American Political Science Review, 80(4), 1095–1111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bacon, A. (2009). Building leaders, living traditions: The Memorial Student Center at Texas A&M University (1st ed.). Texas A&M University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balafoutas, L., & Nikiforakis, N. (2012). Norm enforcement in the city: A natural field experiment. European Economic Review, 56(8), 1773–1785.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Balafoutas, L., Nikiforakis, N., & Rockenbach, B. (2014). Direct and indirect punishment among strangers in the field. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 111(45), 15924–15927.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Balafoutas, L., Nikiforakis, N., & Rockenbach, B. (2016). Altruistic punishment does not increase with the severity of norm violations in the field. Nature Communications, 7, 13327. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms13327

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barclay, P. (2006). Reputational benefits for altruistic punishment. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27(5), 325–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, M., Nettle, D., & Roberts, G. (2006). Cues of being watched enhance cooperation in a real-world setting. Biology Letters, 2(3), 412–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benner, J. (1983). Sul Ross: Soldier, statesman, educator. Texas A&M Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bicchieri, C. (2006). The grammar of society: The nature and dynamics of social norms. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boehm, C. (1999). Hierarchy in the forest: The evolution of egalitarian behavior. Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bowles, S. (1998). Endogenous preferences: The cultural consequences of markets and other economic institutions. Journal of Economic Literature, 36(1), 75–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowles, S. (2004). Microeconomics: Behavior, institutions and evolution. Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowles, S., Boyd, R., Mathew, S., & Richerson, P. J. (2012). The punishment that sustains cooperation is often coordinated and costly. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(1), 20–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. (1990). Group selection among alternative evolutionarily stable strategies. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 145(3), 331–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. (1992). Punishment allows the evolution of cooperation (or anything else) in sizable groups. Ethology and Sociobiology, 13(3), 171–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. (2001). Norms and bounded rationality. In G. Gigerenzer & R. Selten (Eds.), Bounded rationality: The adaptive tool (pp. 281–296). MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. (2008). Gene-culture coevolution and the evolution of social institutions. In E. Christoph & W. Singer (Eds.), Better than conscious? Decision-making, the human mind, and implications for institutions (pp. 305–323). MIT Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., Gintis, H., Bowles, S., & Richerson, P. (2003). The evolution of altruistic punishment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 100(6), 3531–3535.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., Richerson, P., & Henrich, J. (2011). The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 108, 10918–10925.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bronner, J. (2012). Campus traditions: Folklore from the old-time college to the modern mega-university. University Press of Mississippi.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bryan Daily Eagle. (1919). Big crowd witness unveiling of Sul Ross monument. The Bryan Daily Eagle, May 5, A1.

  • Burson, R., & Burson, V. (2004). Reveille: First lady of Texas A&M. Texas A&M University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Centola, D., Becker, J., Brackbill, D., & Baronchelli, A. (2018). Experimental evidence for tipping points in social convention. Science, 360(6393), 1116–1119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chalfin, A., & McCrary, J. (2017). Criminal deterrence: A review of the literature. Journal of Economic Literature, 55(1), 5–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chudek, M., & Henrich, J. (2011). Culture-gene coevolution, norm-psychology and the emergence of human prosociality. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(5), 218–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chudek, M., Heller, S., Birch, S., & Henrich, J. (2012). Prestige-biased cultural learning: Bystander’s differential attention to potential models influences children’s learning. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(1), 46–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coe, K., Aiken, N., & Palmer, C. (2006). Once upon a time: Ancestors and the evolutionary significance of stories. Anthropological Forum, 16(1), 21–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coleman, J. (1990). Foundations of social theory. Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooter, R. (2000). Do good laws make good citizens? An economic analysis of internalized norms. Virginia Law Review, 86(8), 1577–1601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crespi, B., & Summers, K. (2014). Inclusive fitness theory for the evolution of religion. Animal Behaviour, 92, 313–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daiy, K. (2017). The impact of social norms on behavior: Solving the spurious correlation dilemma. Undergraduate research scholars thesis, Texas A&M.

  • Dethloff, H. (1975). A centennial history of Texas A&M University, 2 vols. Texas A&M University Press.

  • Donin, H. (1991). To be a Jew: A guide to Jewish observance in contemporary life. BasicBooks.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dos Santos, M., Rankin, D., & Wedekind, C. (2011). The evolution of punishment through reputation. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278(1704), 371–377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dow, J. (1973). On the muddled concept of corporation in anthropology. American Anthropologist, 75(3), 904–908.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Efferson, C., Lalive, R., Richerson, P., McElreath, R., & Lubell, M. (2008). Conformists and mavericks: The empirics of frequency-dependent cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(1), 56–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellemers, N. (2017). Morality and the regulation of social behavior: Groups as moral anchors. Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2004). Third-party punishment and social norms. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25(2), 63–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., & Gächter, S. (2000). Cooperation and punishment in public goods experiments. American Economic Review, 90(4), 980–994.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., & Gächter, S. (2002). Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature, 415(6868), 137–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • FeldmanHall, O., Sokol-Hessner, P., Van Bavel, J., & Phelps, E. (2014). Fairness violations elicit greater punishment on behalf of another than for oneself. Nature Communications, 5, 5306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fessler, D., Barrett, H. C., Kanovsky, M., Stich, S., Holbrook, C., Henrich, J., . . . Laurence, S. (2015). Moral parochialism and contextual contingency across seven societies. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 282(1813).

  • Fessler, D., Holbrook, C., & Dashoff, D. (2016). Dressed to kill? Visible markers of coalitional affiliation enhance conceptualized formidability. Aggressive Behavior, 42(3), 299–309. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21624

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flood, B. (1999). Historical note: The start of a stop list at biological abstracts. Journal of the American Society for Information Science, 50(12), 1066–1066.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gächter, S. (2012). In the lab and the field: Punishment is rare in equilibrium. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(1), 26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gächter, S., Herrmann, B., & Thöni, C. (2010). Culture and cooperation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 365(1553), 2651–2661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gardiner, J. (2016). Head covering: A forgotten Christian practice for modern times. Alberta, Canada: Head Covering Movement.

  • Gardner, A., & West, S. (2004). Cooperation and punishment, especially in humans. American Naturalist, 164(6), 753–764.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gavrilets, S., & Richerson, P. (2017). Collective action and the evolution of social norm internalization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 114(23), 6068–6073.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gelfand, M., Raver, J. L., Nishii, L., Leslie, L. M., Lun, J., Lim, B. C., & Yamaguchi, S. (2011). Differences between tight and loose cultures: A 33-nation study. Science, 332(6033), 1100–1104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ginges, J., Hansen, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2009). Religion and support for suicide attacks. Psychological Science, 20, 224–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gintis, H. (2003). The hitchhiker’s guide to altruism: Gene-culture coevolution, and the internalization of norms. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 220(4), 407–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giuliano, P., and Nunn, N. (2021). Understanding cultural persistence and change. Review of Economic Studies, 88 (4), 1541–1581. https://scholar.harvard.edu/nunn/publications/understanding-cultural-persistence-and-change.

  • Greif, A. (2006). Institutions and the path to the modern economy: Lessons from medieval trade. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Greif, A., & Kingston, C. (2011). Institutions: Rules or equilibria? In N. Schofield & G. Caballero (Eds.), Political economy of institutions, democracy and voting (pp. 13–43). Springer-Verlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Greif, A., & Laitin, D. D. (2004). A theory of endogenous institutional change. American Political Science Review, 98(4), 633–652.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guala, F. (2012). Strong reciprocity is real, but there is no evidence that uncoordinated costly punishment sustains cooperation in the wild. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(1), 45–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108(4), 814–834.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hasu, P. (2009). For ancestors and god: Rituals of sacrifice among the Chagga of Tanzania. Ethnology, 48(3), 195–213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haun, D., & Over, H. (2015). Like me: A homophily-based account of human culture. In T. Breyer (Ed.), Epistemological dimensions of evolutionary psychology (pp. 117–130). Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J. (2016). The secret of our success: How culture is driving human evolution, domesticating our species, and making us smarter. Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., and Ensminger, J. (2014). Theoretical foundations: The coevolution of social norms, intrinsic motivation, markets, and the institutions of complex societies. In J. Ensminger & J. Henrich (Eds.), Experimenting with social norms: Fairness and punishment in cross-cultural perspective (pp. 19–44). Russell Sage Foundation.

  • Henrich, J., Ensminger, J., McElreath, R., Barr, A., Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., & Ziker, J. (2010). Markets, religion, community size, and the evolution of fairness and punishment. Science, 327(5972), 1480–1484.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., McElreath, R., Barr, A., Ensminger, J., Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., & Ziker, J. (2006). Costly punishment across human societies. Science, 312(5781), 1767–1770.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, B., Thoni, C., & Gächter, S. (2008). Antisocial punishment across societies. Science, 319(5868), 1362–1367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Katz, S., Hediger, M., & Valleroy, L. (1974). Traditional maize processing techniques in the New World. Science, 184(4138), 765–773.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, R. (1985). The Nuer conquest: The structure and development of an expansionist system. University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. (1977). The essential tension: Selected studies in scientific tradition and change. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kurzban, R., DeScioli, P., & O’Brien, E. (2007). Audience effects on moralistic punishment. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28(2), 75–84.

  • Lancaster, J. B. (1986). Primate social behavior and ostracism. Ethology and Sociobiology, 7(3–4), 215–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lapinski, M. K., & Rimal, R. N. (2005). An explication of social norms. Communication Theory, 15(2), 127–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, E., & Go, O. (1997). Survival analysis in public health research. Annual Review of Public Health, 18, 105–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Legros, S., & Cislaghi, B. (2020). Mapping the social-norms literature: An overview of reviews. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15, 62–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacMullen, R. (2010). Christian ancestor worship in Rome. Journal of Biblical Literature, 129(3), 597–613.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maine, H. (1861). Ancient law, its connection with the early history of society, and its relation to modern ideas. John Murray.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mathew, S., & Boyd, R. (2011). Punishment sustains large-scale cooperation in prestate warfare. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 108(28), 11375–11380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maynard-Smith, J., & Szathmary, E. (1995). The major transitions in evolution. W.H. Freeman Spektrum.

    Google Scholar 

  • McElreath, R. (2008). Individual decision making and the evolutionary roots of institutions. In E. Christoph & W. Singer (Eds.), Better than conscious? Decision-making, the human mind, and implications for institutions (pp. 325–342). MIT Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • McWhinney, C. (2014). Incident/investigation report (Case # 14–0226-0012). Texas A&M University Police.

  • Milgram, S. (1977). The individual in a social world: Essays and experiments. Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milgram, S., Liberty, H., Toledo, R., & Wackenhut, J. (1986). Response to intrusion into waiting lines. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(4), 683–689.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nesse, R. (2001). Evolution and the capacity for commitment. Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, M., & Tomaselli, K. (2010). Overimitation in Kalahari Bushman children and the origins of human cultural cognition. Psychological Science, 21(5), 729–736.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, M., Kapitany, R., & Elkins, R. (2015). The perpetuation of ritualistic actions as revealed by young children’s transmission of normative behavior. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(3), 191–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R., & Cohen, D. (1996). Culture of honor: The psychology of violence in the South. Westview Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • North, D. (1990). Institutions, institutional change, and economic performance. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • North, M. (2013). Novelty: A history of the new. University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Nucci, L. P., & Turiel, E. (1978). Social interactions and development of social concepts in preschool children. Child Development, 49(2), 400–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ostrom, E. (1990). Governing the commons: The evolution of institutions for collective action. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Paddon, M. (2007). Southern reveille: Southern culture and tradition at Texas A&M University. Senior scholars thesis. Texas A&M University.

  • Parsons, T. (1951). The social system. Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Post, E. (2004). Emily Post's etiquette (17th ed.). New York HarperCollins.

  • Przepiorka, W., & Berger, J. (2016). The sanctioning dilemma: A quasi-experiment on social norm enforcement in the train. European Sociological Review, 32(3), 439–451. https://doi.org/10.1093/esr/jcw014

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Purzycki, B. G., Apicella, C., Atkinson, Q. D., Cohen, E., McNamara, R. A., Willard, A. K., & Henrich, J. (2016). Moralistic gods, supernatural punishment and the expansion of human sociality. Nature, 530(7590), 327–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rappaport, R. (1979). Ecology, meaning and religion. North Atlantic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rasmussen, S. J. (2000). Alms, elders, and ancestors: The spirit of the gift among the Tuareg. Ethnology, 39(1), 15–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, A. (2014). Man arrested at Texas A&M after allegedly wearing a hat inside student center. The Eagle. College Station, TX.

  • Reed, J. (1982). One South: An ethnic approach to regional culture. Louisiana State University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P., and Boyd, R. (2000). Built for speed: Pleistocene climate variation and the origin of human culture. In F. Tonneau and N. S. Thompson (Eds.), Perspectives in ethology, 13. Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-1221-9_1.

  • Richerson, P., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sayers, M. (2013). Feeding the dead: Ancestor worship in ancient India. Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Schelling, T. (1963). The strategy of conflict. Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schonmann, R., and Boyd, R. (2016). A simple rule for the evolution of contingent cooperation in large groups. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1687).

  • Scott, J. (1971). Internalization of norms. Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheils, D. (1975). Toward a unified theory of ancestor worship: Cross-cultural study. Social Forces, 54(2), 427–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheils, D. (1980). The great ancestors are watching: A cross-cultural-study of superior ancestral religion. Sociological Analysis, 41(3), 247–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shils, E. (1981). Tradition. University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sinclair, S., and Rhockwell, G. (2016). http://voyant-tools.org/. Accessed June 5, 2018.

  • Smith, D. (2020). Cultural group selection and human cooperation: A conceptual and empirical review. Evolutionary Human Sciences, 2(e2), 1–29. https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2020.2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. (2007). The Texas Aggie bonfire: A conservative reading of regional narratives, traditional practices, and a paradoxical place. Annals of the Association of American Geographers, 97(1), 182–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Speiser, E. (1942). The Shibboleth incident (Judges 12:6). Bulletin of the American Schools of Oriental Research, 85, 10–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining culture: A naturalistic approach. Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanford, F. (2003). Escaping the frozen lake: Individual and social idealism manifest as forms of religion and religiosity. Doctoral dissertation, Texas A&M University.

  • Steadman, L., Palmer, C., & Tilley, C. (1996). The universality of ancestor worship. Ethnology, 35(1), 63–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tang, I. (2000). The Texas Aggie bonfire: Tradition and tragedy at Texas A&M. Austin: The It Works.

  • Tatje, T., & Hsu, F. (1969). Variations in ancestor worship beliefs and their relation to kinship. Southwestern Journal of Anthropology, 25(2), 153–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Melis, A. P., Tennie, C., Wyman, E., & Herrmann, E. (2012). Two key steps in the evolution of human cooperation: The interdependence hypothesis. Current Anthropology, 53(6), 673–692.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turiel, E. (1977). Distinct conceptual and developmental domains: Social convention and morality. In C. Keasey (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 77–116). University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., Allan, G., Devlin, S., Kseib, N., Raw, N., and McGuigan, N. (2016). Social learning in the real world: “Over-imitation” occurs in both children and adults unaware of participation in an experiment and independently of social interaction. PLoS ONE, 11(7):e0159920.

  • Wiessner, P. (2005). Norm enforcement among the Ju/’hoansi Bushmen: A case of strong reciprocity? Human Nature, 16(2), 115–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. S. (1990). Weak altruism, strong group selection. Oikos, 59(1), 135–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D., & O’Gorman, R. (2003). Emotions and actions associated with norm-breaking events. Human Nature, 14(3), 277–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Young, H. P. (2015). The evolution of social norms. Annual Review of Economics, 7(1), 359–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

The authors did not receive support from any organization for the submitted work.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael Alvard.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval

This study was performed in line with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki. Approval was granted by the Ethics Committee of Texas A&M University (03MAR14/ IRB2014-0104D).

Conflicts of interest/Competing interests

The authors have no relevant financial or non-financial interests to disclose.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Alvard, M., Daiy, K. A Naturalistic Study of Norm Conformity, Punishment, and the Veneration of the Dead at Texas A&M University, USA. Hum Nat 32, 652–675 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09413-9

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09413-9

Keywords

Navigation