Abstract
Teaching is hypothesized to be a species-typical behavior in humans that contributed to the emergence of cumulative culture. Several within-culture studies indicate that foragers depend heavily on social learning to acquire practical skills and knowledge, but it is unknown whether teaching is universal across forager populations. Teaching can be defined ethologically as the modification of behavior by an expert in the presence of a novice, such that the expert incurs a cost and the novice acquires skills/knowledge more efficiently or that it would not acquire otherwise. One behavioral modification hypothesized to be an adaptation for teaching is ostensive communication—exaggerations of prosody and gesture that signal intent to transmit generalizable knowledge and indicate the intended receiver. On this view, the use of ostensive communication in conjunction with the transmission of generalizable knowledge constitutes evidence of teaching. Oral storytelling appears to meet these criteria: Indigenous peoples regard their traditions as important sources of ecological and social knowledge, and oral storytelling is widely reported to employ paralinguistic communication. To test this hypothesis, descriptions of performed narrative in forager societies were coded for the use of 14 ostensive-communicative behaviors and the presence of generalizable knowledge. Although biased toward North America, the study sample comprised 53 forager cultures spanning five continents, 34 language families, and diverse biomes. All cultures evinced the predicted behaviors. Results suggest that foragers use storytelling as a mode of instruction, thus providing cross-cultural evidence of teaching in forager populations.
Similar content being viewed by others
Data Availability
Author will share upon request.
References
Alvarsson, J. (2012). El individuo y el ambiente—cosmología, etnobiología y etnomedicina. Etnografía ‘weenhayek, 6. Uppsala, Sweden: University of Uppsala and FI’WEN.
Attla, C. (1983). Sitsiy yugh noholnik ts’ in’ = As my grandfather told it: Traditional stories from the Koyukuk. Fairbanks: Yukon Koyukuk School District and Alaska Native Language Center.
Attla, C. (1990). K’etetaalkkaanee: The one who paddled among the people and animals. Fairbanks: Yukon- Koyukuk School District and Alaska Native Language Center.
Bahr, D. (1998). Mythologies compared: Pima, Maricopa, and Yavapai. Journal of the Southwest, 40(1), 25–66.
Basso, K. (1996). Wisdom sits in places: Landscape and language among the Western Apache. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press.
Batchelor, J. (1926). Ainu life and lore. Tokyo: Kyobunkwan.
Berndt, C., & Yunupingu, D. (1979). Land of the Rainbow Snake: Aboriginal children’s stories and songs from western Arnhem Land. Sydney: Collins.
Berndt, R., & Berndt, C. (1994). The speaking land: Myth and story in Aboriginal Australia. Rochester: Inner Traditions International.
Biesele, M. (1978). Sapience and scarce resources: communication systems of the !Kung and other foragers. Social Science Information, 17, 921–947.
Biesele, M. (1993). Women like meat: The folklore and foraging ideology of the Kalahari Ju/’hoan. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
Blurton Jones, N., & Konner, M. (1976). !Kung knowledge of animal behavior. In R. B. Lee & I. DeVore (Eds.), Kalahari hunter-gatherers (pp. 325–348). Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Boas, F. (1898). Introduction. In J. Teit (Ed.), Traditions of the Thompson River Indians of British Columbia (pp. 1–18). Boston: Houghton, Mifflin.
Bogoras, W. (1918). Tales of Yukaghir, Lamut, and Russianized natives of eastern Siberia. Anthropological Papers of the American Museum of Natural History, XX(1), 1–148.
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. (1995). Why does culture increase human adaptability? Ethology and Sociobiology, 16(2), 125–143.
Brand, R., Baldwin, D., & Ashburn, L. (2002). Evidence for “motionese”: Modifications in mothers’ infant-directed action. Developmental Science, 5(1), 72–83.
Brand, R. J., & Shallcross, W. K. (2008). Infants prefer motionese to adult-directed action. Developmental Science, 11, 853–861.
Brown, D. (1991). Human universals. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Burch, E. Jr. (1991). From skeptic to believer: the making of an oral historian. Alaska History, 6(1), 1–16.
Burch, E. Jr. (2005). Alliance and conflict: The world system of the Iñupiaq Eskimos. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Byrne, R. (1995). The thinking ape: Evolutionary origins of intelligence. New York: Oxford University Press.
Caporael, L. R. (1981). The paralanguage of caregiving: Baby talk to the institutionalized aged. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40(5), 876–884.
Caro, R., & Hauser, M. (1992). Is there teaching in non-human animals? Quarterly Review of Biology, 67(2), 151–174.
Clark, E. (1966). Indian legends from the northern Rockies. Norman: University of Oklahoma Press.
Collins, C. (2013). Paleopoetics. New York: Columbia University Press.
Cooper, R., & Aslin, R. (1990). Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth. Child Development, 61(5), 1584–1595.
Cowell, A., Moss, A., & C’Hair, W. (2014). Arapaho stories, songs, and prayers: A bilingual anthology. Norman: University of Oklahoma Press.
Cruikshank, J. (1990). Life lived like a story. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2006). Social learning and social cognition: The case for pedagogy. Processes of change in brain and cognitive development. Attention and Performance XXI(21), 249–274.
Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2009). Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(4), 148–153.
Culberston, G., & Caporael, L. (1983). Babytalk speech to the elderly: complexity and content of messages. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 9(2), 305–312.
de Laguna, F. (1995). Tales from the Dena. Seattle: University of Washington Press.
de Wilde, I., & de Bot, K. (1989). Taal van verzorgenden tegen ouderen in een psychogeriatrisch verpleeghuis. Tijdschrift Gerontologie und Geriatrigie, 20, 97–100.
Dean, L., Kendal, R., Schapiro, S., Thierry, B., & Laland, K. (2012). Identification of the social and cognitive processes underlying human cumulative culture. Science, 335(6072), 1114–1118.
Deloria, E. (1932). Dakota texts. Publications of the American Ethnological Society, XIV. New York: G. E. Stechert and Co.
Deur, D., & Turner, N. (Eds.). (2005). Keeping it living: Traditions of plant use and cultivation on the Northwest Coast of North America. Seattle: University of Washington Press.
Devens, C. (1994). Anishnabek childhood: Nineteenth and early twentieth centuries. Michigan Historical Review, 20(2), 184–197.
Dietrich, O., Notroff, J., & Schmidt, K. (2017). Feasting, social complexity, and the emergence of the early Neolithic of Upper Mesopotamia: A view from Göbekli Tepe. In R. Chacon & R. Mendoza (Eds.), Feast, famine or fighting? (pp. 91–132). Cham: Springer.
Dissanayake, E. (1995). Chimera, spandrel, or adaptation? Conceptualizing art in human evolution. Human Nature, 6(2), 99–117.
Dixon, R. (1910). Shasta myths. Journal of American Folklore, 23(87), 8–37.
Dolitsky, A. B. (2012). A critical review of the traditional narratives of Chukotka and Kamchatka. Sibirica, 11(3), 20–55.
Elder, W. (1871). The aborigines of Nova Scotia. The North American Review, 112(1), 1–30.
Elmendorf, W. (1961). Skokomish and other Coast Salish Tales (Parts I-III). Research Studies: A Quarterly Publication of Washington State University, XXIX, 1–37(84–117), 119–150.
Falk, D. (2004). Prelinguistic evolution in early hominins: Whence motherese? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(4), 491–503.
Fernald, A. (1985). Four-month-old infants prefer to listen to motherese. Infant Behavior and Development, 8(2), 181–195.
Fernald, A. (1989). Intonation and communicative intent in mothers’ speech to infants: Is the melody the message? Child Development, 60(6), 1497–1510.
Fernald, A. (1992). Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: an evolutionary perspective. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind (pp. 391–428). New York: Oxford University Press.
Fernald, A., & Kuhl, P. (1987). Acoustic determinants of infant preference for motherese speech. Infant Behavior and Development, 10(3), 279–293.
Gifford, E. (1917). Miwok myths. University of California Publications in American Archaeology and Ethnology, 12(8), 283–338.
Grainger, K. (1993). “That’s a lovely bath dear”: Reality construction in the discourse of elderly care. Journal of Aging Studies, 7(3), 247–262.
Gwich’in Elders. (1997). Gwich’in words about the land. Inuvik: Gwich’in Renewable Resource Board.
Headland, T., Reid, L., Bicchieri, M., Bishop, C., Blust, R., Flanders, N., . . . Seitz, S. (1989). Hunter-gatherers and their neighbors from prehistory to the present. Current Anthropology, 30(1), 43–66.
Hewlett, B., & Cavalli-Sforza, L. (1986). Cultural transmission among Aka pygmies. American Anthropologist, 88(4), 922–934.
Hewlett, B., & Roulette, C. (2016). Teaching in hunter-gatherer infancy. Royal Society Open Science, 3(1), 150403.
Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Treiman, R. (1982). Doggerel: Motherese in a new context. Journal of Child Language, 9(1), 229–237.
Howell, R. W. (1951). The classification and description of Ainu folklore. The Journal of American Folklore, 64(254), 361–369.
Hymes, D. (1981). “In vain I tried to tell you”: Essays on Native American ethnopoetics. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Hymes, D. (2003). Now I know only so far: Essays in ethnopoetics. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Ikeda, Y., & Masataka, N. (1999). A variable that may affect individual differences in the child-directed speech of Japanese women. Japanese Psychological Research, 41(4), 203–208.
Jacobs, M. (1959). The content and style of an oral literature: Clackamas Chinook myths and tales. New York: Wenner-Gren Foundation for Anthropological Research.
Jetté, J. (1908). On Ten’a folk-lore. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 38(11), 298–367.
Jetté, J. (1909). On Ten’a folk-lore (Part II). Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 39(12), 460–505.
Johnson, L. (2010). Trail of story, traveller’s path: Reflections on ethnoecology and landscape. Edmonton: AU Press, Athabasca University.
Kaplan, H., & Hill, K. (1992). The evolutionary ecology of food acquisition. In E. Smith & B. Winterhalder (Eds.), Evolutionary ecology and human behavior (pp. 167–201). New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
Kaplan, H., Hill, K., Lancaster, J., & Hurtado, A. (2000). A theory of human life history evolution: Diet, intelligence, and longevity. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9(4), 156–185.
Kline, M. (2017). Teach: An ethogram-based method to observe and record teaching behavior. Field Methods, 29(3), 205–220.
Kroeber, A. (1948). Seven Mohave myths. Anthropological Records, 11(1), 1–70. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Kroeber, A., & Gifford, E. (1980). Karok myths. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Laland, K. (2004). Social learning strategies. Animal Learning and Behavior, 32(1), 4–14.
Laughlin, W. (1968). Hunting: An integrating biobehavior system and its evolutionary importance. In R. Lee & I. DeVore (Eds.), Man the hunter (pp. 304–320). New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
Leacock, E. (1954). The Montagnais “hunting territory” and the fur trade. Menasha: American Anthropological Association.
Lee, R., & DeVore, I. (1968). Problems in the study of hunters and gatherers. In R. Lee & I. DeVore (Eds.), Man the hunter (pp. 3–12). New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
Legat, A., & Barnaby, J. (2012). Walking the land, feeding the fire. Tucson: University of Arizona Press.
Lewis, H. (1982). Fire technology and resource management in aboriginal North America and Australia. In N. Williams & E. Hunn (Eds.), Resource managers: North American and Australian hunter-gatherers (pp. 45–68). Boulder: Westview Press for the American Association for the Advancement of Science.
Lewis-Williams, J. (2000). Stories that float from afar: Ancestral folklore of the San of southern Africa. College Station: Texas A and M University Press.
Lew-Levy, S., Crittenden, A., Boyette, A., Mabulla, I., Hewlett, B. S., & Lamb, M. (2019). Inter- and intra-cultural variation in learning-through-participation among Hadza and BaYaka forager children and adolescents from Tanzania and Congo. Journal of Psychology in Africa, 29(4), 309–318.
Lew-Levy, S., Reckin, R., Lavi, N., Cristóbal-Azkarate, J., & Ellis-Davies, K. (2017). How do hunter-gatherer children learn subsistence skills? A meta-ethnographic review. Human Nature, 28(4), 367–394.
Lord, A. (1960). The singer of tales. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Lowie, R. (1918). Myths and traditions of the Crow Indians. Anthropological Papers of the American Museum of Natural History, XXV(1). New York.
Marlowe, F. (2005). Hunter-gatherers and human evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology, 14(2), 54–67.
Masataka, N. (1998). Perception of motherese in Japanese sign language by 6-month-old hearing infants. Developmental Psychology, 34(2), 241–246.
Masataka, N. (2002). Pitch modification when interacting with elders: Japanese women with and without experience with infants. Journal of Child Language, 29(4), 939–951.
Massola, A. (1968). Bunjil’s Cave: Myths legends and superstitions of the Aborigines of south-east Australia. Melbourne: Lansdowne Press.
Mazuka, R., Kondo, T., & Hayashi, A. (2008). Japanese mothers’ use of specialized vocabulary in infant-directed speech: Infant-directed vocabulary in Japanese. In N. Masataka (Ed.), The origins of language (pp. 39–58). Tokyo: Springer.
McClellan, C. (1987). Part of the land, part of the water: A history of the Yukon Indians. Vancouver: Douglas and McIntyre.
Miall, D., & Dissanayake, E. (2003). The poetics of babytalk. Human Nature, 14(4), 337–364.
Miller, G. (2000). The mating mind: How sexual choice shaped the evolution of human nature. New York: Doubleday.
Minc, L. (1986). Scarcity and survival: the role of oral tradition in mediating subsistence crises. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 5(1), 39–113.
Mithen, S. (2003). After the ice. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Mithen, S. (2006). Ethnobiology and the evolution of the human mind. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 12, S45–S61.
Moss, M. (2005). Tlingit horticulture. In D. Deur & N. Turner (Eds.), Keeping it living (pp. 274–295). Seattle: University of Washington Press.
Murdock, G. (1967). Ethnographic atlas. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press.
Napaljarri, P., & Cataldi, L. (1994). Warlpiri dreamings and histories. San Francisco: Harper Collins.
Nelson, R. (1969). Hunters of the northern ice. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Ohmagari, K., & Berkes, F. (1997). Transmission of indigenous knowledge and bush skills among the western James Bay Cree women of subarctic Canada. Human Ecology, 25(2), 197–222.
Ong, W. (1982). Orality and literacy. London: Routledge.
Opler, M. (1938). Myths and tales of the Jicarilla Apache Indians. Memoirs of the American Folk-lore Society, XXXI. New York: G.E. Stechert.
Ortiz Lema, E. (1986). Los Mataco Noctenes de Bolivia. La Paz-Cochabamba: Editorial Los Amigos del Libro.
Parsons, E. (1925). Micmac folklore. Journal of American Folklore, 38(147), 55–133.
Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. New York: W.W. Norton.
Radin, P. (1915). Literary aspects of North American mythology. Canada Geological Survey Museum Bulletin No. 16. Ottawa: Government Printing Bureau.
Radin, P. (1926). Literary aspects of Winnebago mythology. Journal of American Folklore, 39(151), 18–52.
Ramsey, J. (1977). Coyote was going there: Indian literature of the Oregon country. Seattle: University of Washington Press.
Rasmussen, K., & Ostermann, H. (1952). The Alaskan Eskimos as described in the posthumous notes of Knud Rasmussen. Copenhagen: Gyldendal.
Reichard, G. (1947). An analysis of Coeur d’Alene Indian myths. Memoirs of the American Folklore Society, XLI. New York: American Folklore Society.
Ridington, R. (1988). Trail to heaven: Knowledge and narrative in a northern Native community. Iowa City: University of Iowa Press.
Rogers, A. (1988). Does biology constrain culture? American Anthropologist, 90(4), 819–831.
Sachweh, S. (1998). Granny darling’s nappies: Secondary babytalk in German nursing homes for the aged. Journal of Applied Communication Research: Applied Research in Language and Intergenerational Communication, 26(1), 52–65.
Sapir, E. (1910). Song recitative in Paiute mythology. Journal of American Folklore, 23(90), 455–472.
Sapir, E., & Curtin, J. (1909). Wishram texts. Leyden: E.J. Brill.
Sapir, E., & Dixon, R. B. (1910). Yana texts. University of California Publications in American Archaeology and Ethnology, 9(1), 1–235.
Scalise Sugiyama, M. (1996). On the origins of narrative: Storyteller bias as a fitness-enhancing strategy. Human Nature, 7(4), 403–425.
Scalise Sugiyama, M. (2017a). Oral storytelling as evidence of pedagogy in forager societies. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, article 471.
Scalise Sugiyama, M. (2017b). Narrative. In T. Shackelford and V. Weekes-Shackelford (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science. Berlin: Springer. https://link.springer.com/referenceworkentry/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_3316-1.
Scalise Sugiyama, M., Mendoza, M., & Quiroz, I. (2020). Ethnobotanical knowledge encoded in Weenhayek oral tradition. Journal of Ethnobiology, 40(1), 39–55.
Scheub, H. (1977). Body and image in oral narrative performance. New Literary History, 8(3), 345–367.
Schniter, E., Gurven, M., Kaplan, H., Wilcox, N., & Hooper, P. (2015). Skill ontogeny among Tsimane forager-horticulturalists. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 158(1), 3–18.
Scott, S., & Ellis, C. & Algonquian Text Society. (1995). Âtalôhkâna nêsta tipâcimôwina = Cree legends and narratives from the west coast of James Bay. Winnipeg: University of Manitoba Press.
Seaburg, W. (Ed.). (2007). Pitch Woman and other stories. Collected by E. D. Jacobs. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Shelton, J. (1998). As the Romans did: A sourcebook in Roman social history (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Shipley, W. (1991). The Maidu Indian myths and stories of Hanc’ibyjim. Berkeley: Heyday Books.
Shute, B., & Wheldall, K. (1989). Pitch alternations in British motherese: Some preliminary acoustic data. Journal of Child Language, 16(3), 503–512.
Shute, B., & Wheldall, K. (1995). The incidence of raised average pitch and increased pitch variability of British motherese speech and the influence of maternal characteristics and discourse form. First Language, 15(1), 35–55.
Skinner, A., & Satterlee, J. (1915). Folklore of the Menomini Indians. Anthropological Papers of the American Museum of Natural History, 8(3), 223–544.
Smith, D., Schlaepfer, P., Major, K., Dyble, M., Page, A., Thompson, J. . . . Migliano, A. (2017). Cooperation and the evolution of hunter-gatherer storytelling. Nature Communications, 8(1), 1853.
Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1986). Relevance: communication and cognition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Stern, B. (1934). The Lummi Indians of Northwest Washington. New York: Columbia University Press.
Stern, T. (1956). Some sources of variability in Klamath mythology. Journal of American Folklore, 69(271), 1–12.
Stiles, D. (2001). Hunter-gatherer studies: The importance of context. African Study Monographs, 26(Suppl.), 41–65.
Strauss, S., & Ziv, M. (2012). Teaching is a natural cognitive ability for humans. Mind, Brain, and Education, 6(4), 186–196.
Strauss, S., Ziv, M., & Stein, A. (2002). Teaching as a natural cognition and its relations to preschoolers’ developing theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 17(3–4), 1473–1487.
Sugiyama, L. (2015). Physical attractiveness: An adaptationist perspective. In D. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 317–384). Hoboken: Wiley.
Tedlock, D. (1971). On the translation of style in oral narrative. Journal of American Folklore, 84(331), 114–133.
Tedlock, D. (1977). Toward an oral poetics. New Literary History, 8(3), 507–519.
Tenenbaum, J., & McGary, M. (1984). Dena’ina sukdu’a: Traditional stories of the Tanaina Athabaskans. Fairbanks: Alaska Native Language Center.
Tennant, E., & Bitar, J. (1981). Yupik lore: Oral traditions of an Eskimo people. Bethel: Lower Kuskokwim School District.
Thompson, S. (1929). Tales of the North American Indians. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
Thornton, A., & Raihani, N. J. (2008). The evolution of teaching. Animal Behaviour, 75(6), 1823–1836.
Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A., & Ratner, H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16(3), 495–511.
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1990). The past explains the present: Emotional adaptations and the structure of ancestral environments. Ethology and Sociobiology, 11(4–5), 375–424.
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 19–136). New York: Oxford University Press.
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2001). Does beauty build adapted minds? Toward an evolutionary theory of aesthetics, fiction and the arts. SubStance, 30(1/2), 6–27.
Tooby, J., & DeVore, I. (1987). The reconstruction of hominid behavioral evolution through strategic modeling. In W. Kinzey (Ed.), The evolution of human behavior: Primate models (pp. 183–237). Albany: SUNY Press.
Turnbull, C. (1959). Legends of the BaMbuti. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland, 89(1), 45–60.
Turner, N. (2014). Ancient pathways, ancestral knowledge. McGill-Queen’s Native and Northern Series, Vol. 74. Montreal: McGill-Queen’s Press.
Whiten, A. (2000). Primate culture and social learning. Cognitive Science, 24(3), 477–508.
Wiessner, P. (2014). Embers of society: Firelight talk among the Ju/’hoansi Bushmen. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(39), 14027–14035.
Wilbert, J., & Simoneau, K. (1990). Folk literature of the Yanomami Indians. Los Angeles: UCLA Latin American Center Publications.
Yamada, T. (2001). The world view of the Ainu. London: Kegan Paul.
Zegwaard, G., Pouwer, J., & Offenberg, G. (2000). Amoko—In the beginning: Myths and legends of the Asmat and Mimika Papuans. Adelaide: Crawford Publishing House.
Zwirner, E., & Thornton, A. (2015). Cognitive requirements of cumulative culture: Teaching is useful but not essential. Scientific Reports, 5(1), 1–8.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of Interest
The author declares no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
ESM 1
(PDF 208 KB)
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Scalise Sugiyama, M. Co‐occurrence of Ostensive Communication and Generalizable Knowledge in Forager Storytelling. Hum Nat 32, 279–300 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09385-w
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09385-w