Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Can Development Programs Shape Cooperation?

Results from a Framed Field Experiment in Indonesia

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Empirical studies among small-scale societies show that participation in national development programs impact traditional norms of community cooperation. We explore the extent to which varying levels of village and individual involvement in development policies relate to voluntary cooperation within community settings. We used a field experiment conducted in seven villages (208 participants) from an indigenous society in Indonesia known for their strong traditional cooperative norms, the Punan Tubu. We framed the experiment in terms of an ongoing government house-building program. The results indicate that there were synergistic and antagonistic interactions between existing cooperative norms and government development policies. Participants’ cooperation in the experimental setting was low, probably because the Punan Tubu are used to cooperating and sharing both under demand and in a context in which uncooperative behavior is largely unpunished. Variation in experimental behavior was related to both village- and individual-level variables, with participants living in resettlement villages and participants living in a house constructed under the government program displaying more cooperative behavior. The cooperation evident in resettled villages may indicate that people in these villages are more comfortable interacting in anonymous settings and less committed to the demand-sharing norms still prevalent in the upstream villages. The more cooperative behavior among villagers who have previously received a house might indicate that they recognize that they are now better off than others and feel more obliged to cooperate. Policies aiming to capitalize on existing cooperative behavior to stimulate community collective action should consider the specific conditions under which cooperation occurs in real settings since traditional norms that regulate cooperative behavior might not translate well to cooperation in government-led programs.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Agrawal, A. (2005). Environmentality. Current Anthropology, 46(2), 161–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Agrawal, A., & Gibson, C. C. (1999). Enchantment and disenchantment: The role of community in natural resource conservation. World Development, 27(4), 629–649.

    Google Scholar 

  • Antlöv, H., Wetterberg, A., & Dharmawan, L. (2016). Village governance, community life, and the 2014 village law in Indonesia. Bulletin of Indonesian Economic Studies, 52(2), 161–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beedell, J., & Rehman, T. (2000). Using social-psychology models to understand farmers’ conservation behaviour. Journal of Rural Studies, 16(1), 117–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bigoni, M., Bortolotti, S., Casari, M., Gambetta, D., & Pancotto, F. (2016). Amoral familism, social capital, or trust? The behavioural foundations of the Italian north-south divide. The Economic Journal, 126(594), 1318–1341.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boehm, C. (1999). Hierarchy in the forest: The evolution of egalitarian behavior. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bouma, J. A., & Ansink, E. (2013). The role of legitimacy perceptions in self-restricted resource use: A framed field experiment. Forest Policy and Economics, 37, 84–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bouma, J. A., Joy, K., Paranjape, S., & Ansink, E. (2014). The influence of legitimacy perceptions on cooperation – A framed field experiment. World Development, 57, 127–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowles, S. (1998). Endogenous preferences: The cultural consequences of markets and other economic institutions. Journal of Economic Literature, 36(1), 75–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cárdenas, J.-C. (2000). How do groups solve local commons dilemmas? Lessons from experimental economics in the field. Environment, Development and Sustainability, 2(3–4), 305–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cardenas, J. C., & Carpenter, J. (2008). Behavioural development economics: Lessons from field labs in the developing world. The Journal of Development Studies, 44(3), 311–338.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cárdenas, J.-C., & Ostrom, E. (2004). What do people bring into the game? Experiments in the field about cooperation in the commons. Agricultural Systems, 82(3), 307–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cárdenas, J.-C., Janssen, M. A., Ale, M., Bastakoti, R., Bernal, A., Chalermphol, J., Gong, Y., Shin, H., Shivakoti, G., Wang, Y., & Anderies, J. M. (2017). Fragility of the provision of local public goods to private and collective risks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(5), 921–925.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chaudhuri, A. (2011). Sustaining cooperation in laboratory public goods experiments: A selective survey of the literature. Experimental Economics, 14, 47–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cronk, L. (2007). The influence of cultural framing on play in the trust game: A Maasai example. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28(5), 352–358.

    Google Scholar 

  • Croson, R., & Gneezy, U. (2009). Differences in preferences. Journal of Economic Literature, 47(2), 448–474.

    Google Scholar 

  • Curtis, A., Britton, A., & Sobels, J. (1999). Landcare networks in Australia: State-sponsored participation through local organizations. Journal of Environmental Planning and Management, 42(1), 5–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Curtis, A., Ross, H., Marshall, G. R., Baldwin, C., Cavaye, J., Freeman, C., Carr, A., & Syme, G. J. (2014). The great experiment with devolved NRM governance: Lessons from community engagement in Australia and New Zealand since the 1980s. Australasian Journal of Environmental Management, 21(2), 175–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dasgupta, A., & Beard, V. A. (2007). Community-driven development, collective action and elite capture in Indonesia. Development and Change, 38(2), 229–249.

  • Dasgupta, P., & Serageldin, I. (2000). Social capital: A multifaceted perspective. World Bank.

  • Dufwenberg, M., Gächter, S., & Hennig-Schmidt, H. (2011). The framing of games and the psychology of play. Games and Economic Behavior, 73(2), 459–478.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., Fischbacher, U., & Gächter, S. (2002). Strong reciprocity, human cooperation, and the enforcement of social norms. Human Nature, 13(1), 1–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fielding, K. S., Terry, D. J., Masser, B. M., Bordia, P., & Hogg, M. A. (2005). Explaining landholders’ decisions about riparian zone management: The role of behavioural, normative, and control beliefs. Journal of Environmental Management, 77(1), 12–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gneezy, U., Leibbrandt, A., & List, J. A. (2016). Ode to the sea: Workplace organizations and norms of cooperation. The Economic Journal, 126(595), 1856–1883.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gurven, M., & Winking, J. (2008). Collective action in action: Prosocial behavior in and out of the laboratory. American Anthropologist, 110(2), 179–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, G. W., & List, J. A. (2004). Field experiments. Journal of Economic Literature, 42(4), 1009–1055.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrisson, T. (1975). Further notes on Sarawak and Kalimantan Punan (and Penan). Borneo Research Bulletin, 7. http://cpasru.nl/node/12074. Accessed 10 September 2019.

  • Hawkes, K. (1992). Sharing and collective action. In E. A. Smith & B. Winterhalder (Eds.), Evolutionary ecology and human behavior (pp. 269–300). Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C., Fehr, E., Gintis, H., McElreath, R., Alvard, M., Barr, A., Ensminger, J., Henrich, N. S., Hill, K., Gil-White, F., Gurven, M., Marlowe, F. W., Patton, J. Q., & Tracer, D. (2005). “Economic man” in cross-cultural perspective: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(06), 795–815, 815–55.

  • Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C., Fehr, E., Gintis, H., & McElreath, R. (2001). In search of Homo oeconomicus: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. American Economic Review, 91(2), 73–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., Ensminger, J., McElreath, R., Barr, A., Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., Cardenas, J. C., Gurven, M., Gwako, E., Henrich, N., Lesorogol, C., Marlowe, F., Tracer, D., & Ziker, J. (2010). Markets, religion, community size, and the evolution of fairness and punishment. Science, 327(5972), 1480–1484.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., McElreath, R., Barr, A., Ensminger, J., Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., Cardenas, J. C., Gurven, M., Gwako, E., Henrich, N., Lesorogol, C., Marlowe, F., Tracer, D., & Ziker, J. (2006). Costly punishment across human societies. Science, 312(5781), 1767–1770.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K. (2002). Altruistic cooperation during foraging by the Ache, and the evolved human predisposition to cooperate. Human Nature, 13(1), 105–128.

  • Kameda, T., Takezawa, M., & Hastie, R. (2005). Where do social norms come from? The example of communal sharing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(6), 331–334.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, H. S., & Gurven, M. (2005). The natural history of human food sharing and cooperation: A review and a new multi-individual approach to the negotiation of norms. In H. Gintis, R. Boyd, & E. Fehr (Eds.), Moral sentiments and material interests: The foundations of cooperation in economic life (pp. 75–14). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaskija, L. (2012). Images of a forest people: Punan Malinau identity, sociality, and encapsulation in Borneo. Uppsala Studies in Cultural Anthropology 52. Uppsala, Sweden: Uppsala Universitet.

  • Kasper, C., & Mulder, M. B. (2015). Who helps and why? Current Anthropology, 56(5), 701–732.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ledyard, J. O. (1994). Public goods: A survey of experimental research. Public Economics. https://ideas.repec.org/p/wpa/wuwppe/9405003.html. Accessed 10 September 2019.

  • Lesorogol, C. K. (2007). Bringing norms in. Current Anthropology, 48(6), 920–926.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levang, P., Sitorus, S., & Dounias, E. (2007). City life in the midst of the forest: A Punan hunter-gatherer’s vision of conservation and development. Ecology and Society, 12(1), 18 http://www.ecologyandsociety.org/vol12/iss1/art18/.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, H. M., Vinicius, L., Strods, J., Mace, R., & Migliano, A. B. (2014). High mobility explains demand sharing and enforced cooperation in egalitarian hunter-gatherers. Nature Communications, 5(1), 6789.

    Google Scholar 

  • Napitupulu, L., Bouma, J., & Reyes-García, V. (2018). The things we share: Sharing in daily life and experimental settings among Punan Tubu, Indonesian Borneo. Ecological Economics, 152, 88–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Napitupulu, L., Guèze, M., & Reyes-García, V. (2016). Sharing in a context of rural development. A study among a contemporary hunter-gatherer society in Indonesia. In V. Reyes-García & A. Pyhälä (Eds.), Hunter-gatherers in a changing world (pp. 127–147). Cham: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-42271-8_8.

  • Narloch, U., Pascual, U., & Drucker, A. G. (2012). Collective action dynamics under external rewards: Experimental insights from Andean farming communities. World Development, 40(10), 2096–2107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Onyeiwu, S., & Jones, R. (2003). An institutionalist perception of cooperative behavior. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 32(3), 233–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ostrom, E. (1990). Governing the commons: The evolution of institutions for collective action. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ostrom, E. (2000). Collective action and the evolution of social norms. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 14(3), 137–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ostrom, E. (2006). The value-added of laboratory experiments for the study of institutions and common-pool resources. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 61(2), 149–163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palfrey, T. R., & Prisbrey, J. E. (1997). Anomalous behavior in public goods experiments: How much and why? The American Economic Review, 87(5), 829–846.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patton, J. Q. (2005). Meat sharing for coalitional support. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(2), 137–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • PEMKAB Malinau. (2013). Pedoman Gerakan Desa Membangun (Guidelines of Gerdema). Malinau, Indonesia.

  • Purzycki, B. G., Henrich, J., Apicella, C., Atkinson, Q. D., Baimel, A., Cohen, E., McNamara, R. A., Willard, A. K., Xygalatas, D., & Norenzayan, A. (2018). The evolution of religion and morality: A synthesis of ethnographic and experimental evidence from eight societies. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 8(2), 101–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Race, D., & Sumirat, B. (2015). Exploring the implications of social inequalities in community forestry: Emerging lessons from two forests in Indonesia. International Journal of Sustainable Development, 18(3), 211–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sellato, B. (1994). Nomads of the Borneo rainforest: The economics, politics, and ideology of settling down. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sercombe, P., & Sellato, B. (Eds.). (2007). Beyond the green myth: Borneo’s hunter-gatherers in the twenty-first century. Nordic Institute of Asian Studies (NIAS): Copenhagen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shariff, A. F., Willard, A. K., Andersen, T., & Norenzayan, A. (2016). Religious priming. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 20(1), 27–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vollan, B. (2012). Pitfalls of externally initiated collective action: A case study from South Africa. World Development, 40(4), 758–770.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wollenberg, E. K., Iwan, R., Limberg, G., Moeliono, M., Rhee, S., & Sudana, I. M. (2007). Facilitating cooperation during times of chaos: Spontaneous orders and muddling through in Malinau district, Indonesia. Ecology and Society, 12(1), 3 https://www.ecologyandsociety.org/vol12/iss1/art3/.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was funded by the European Research Council under the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007-2013)/ERC grant agreement FP7-261971-LEK to V. Reyes-García. We express our profound gratitude to all the informants for their willingness to share their time and knowledge. We thank the WWF Indonesia-Malinau, the Department of Economics Universitas Indonesia, and the CIFOR-Bogor for facilitating fieldwork. We also thank M. Guèze, Jonaidi, Widya NP, S. Hadiwijaya, and D. Suan for research assistance at different stages of fieldwork. The authors acknowledge financial support from the Spanish Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities, through the “María de Maeztu” program for Units of Excellence (MDM-2015-0552).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Victoria Reyes-García.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

ESM 1

(PDF 414 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Napitupulu, L., Bouma, J., Graham, S. et al. Can Development Programs Shape Cooperation?. Hum Nat 31, 174–195 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-020-09369-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-020-09369-2

Keywords

Navigation