Advertisement

Human Nature

, Volume 28, Issue 1, pp 53–75 | Cite as

The Relative Importance of Sexual Dimorphism, Fluctuating Asymmetry, and Color Cues to Health during Evaluation of Potential Partners’ Facial Photographs

A Conjoint Analysis Study
  • Justin K. Mogilski
  • Lisa L. M. Welling
Article

Abstract

Sexual dimorphism, symmetry, and coloration in human faces putatively signal information relevant to mate selection and reproduction. Although the independent contributions of these characteristics to judgments of attractiveness are well established, relatively few studies have examined whether individuals prioritize certain features over others. Here, participants (N = 542, 315 female) ranked six sets of facial photographs (3 male, 3 female) by their preference for starting long- and short-term romantic relationships with each person depicted. Composite-based digital transformations were applied such that each image set contained 11 different versions of the same identity. Each photograph in each image set had a unique combination of three traits: sexual dimorphism, symmetry, and color cues to health. Using conjoint analysis to evaluate participants’ ranking decisions, we found that participants prioritized cues to sexual dimorphism over symmetry and color cues to health. Sexual dimorphism was also found to be relatively more important for the evaluation of male faces than for female faces, whereas symmetry and color cues to health were relatively more important for the evaluation of female faces than for male faces. Symmetry and color cues to health were more important for long-term versus short-term evaluations for female faces, but not male faces. Analyses of utility estimates reveal that our data are consistent with research showing that preferences for facial masculinity and femininity in male and female faces vary according to relationship context. These findings are interpreted in the context of previous work examining the influence of these facial attributes on romantic partner perception.

Keywords

Sexual dimorphism Symmetry Health Conjoint analysis Faces Trade-offs 

References

  1. Abbas, Z. A., & Duchaine, B. (2008). The role of holistic processing in judgments of facial attractiveness. Perception, 37, 1187–1196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Armstrong, N., & Welsman, J. (2001). Peak oxygen uptake in relation to growth and maturation in 11- to 17- year-old humans. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 85, 546–551.Google Scholar
  3. Baudouin, J. Y., & Tiberghien, G. (2004). Symmetry, averageness, and feature size in the facial attractiveness of women. Acta Psychologica, 117, 313–332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Biederman, I. (1987). Recognition-by-components: a theory of human image understanding. Psychological Review, 94, 115–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Boothroyd, L. G., Jones, B. C., Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2007). Partner characteristics associated with masculinity, health and maturity in male faces. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 1161–1173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Boothroyd, L. G., Jones, B. C., Burt, D. M., DeBruine, L. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2008). Facial correlates of sociosexuality. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 211–218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Boothroyd, L. G., Lawson, J. F., & Michael Burt, D. (2009). Testing immunocompetence explanations of male facial masculinity. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 7, 65–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: an evolutionary perspective on human mating. Psychological Review, 100, 204–232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Charkoudian, N., Stephens, D. P., Pirkle, K. C., Kosiba, W. A., & Johnson, J. M. (1999). Influence of female reproductive hormones on local thermal control of skin blood flow. Journal of Applied Physiology, 87, 1719–1723.Google Scholar
  10. Cunningham, M. R., Roberts, A. R., Barbee, A. P., Druen, P. B., & Wu, C. H. (1995). “Their ideas of beauty are, on the whole, the same as ours”: consistency and variability in the cross-cultural perception of female physical attractiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 261–279.Google Scholar
  11. DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Boothroyd, L. G., Perrett, D. I., Penton-Voak, I. S., et al. (2006). Correlated preferences for facial masculinity and ideal or actual partner’s masculinity. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 273, 1355–1360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., Crawford, J. R., Welling, L. L., & Little, A. C. (2010a). The health of a nation predicts their mate preferences: cross-cultural variation in women’s preferences for masculinized male faces. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 277, 2405–2410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., Smith, F. G., & Little, A. C. (2010b). Are attractive men’s faces masculine or feminine? The importance of controlling confounds in face stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 751–758.Google Scholar
  14. DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Crawford, J. R., & Welling, L. L. (2011). Further evidence for regional variation in women’s masculinity preferences. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 283, 1–2.Google Scholar
  15. Feinberg, D. R., Jones, B. C., Smith, M. L., Moore, F. R., DeBruine, L. M., Cornwell, R. E., et al. (2006). Menstrual cycle, trait estrogen level, and masculinity preferences in the human voice. Hormones and Behavior, 49, 215–222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Fink, B., & Penton-Voak, I. (2002). Evolutionary psychology of facial attractiveness. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 154–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Fink, B., Grammer, K., & Matts, P. J. (2006). Visible skin color distribution plays a role in the perception of age, attractiveness, and health in female faces. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27, 433–442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Fink, B., Neave, N., & Seydel, H. (2007). Male facial appearance signals physical strength to women. American Journal of Human Biology, 19, 82–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Fink, B., Matts, P. J., D’Emiliano, D., Bunse, L., Weege, B., & Röder, S. (2012). Colour homogeneity and visual perception of age, health and attractiveness of male facial skin. Journal of the European Academy of Dermatology and Venereology, 26, 1486–1492.Google Scholar
  20. Fink, B., Weege, B., Neave, N., Ried, B., & Do Lago, O. C. (2014). Female perceptions of male body movements. In V. A. Weekes-Shackelford & T. K. Shackelford (Eds.), Evolutionary perspectives on human sexual psychology and behavior (pp. 297–322). NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Fink, B., Weege, B., Neave, N., Pham, M. N., & Shackelford, T. K. (2015). Integrating body movement into attractiveness research. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Folstad, I., & Karter, A. J. (1992). Parasites, bright males, and the immunocompetence handicap. American Naturalist, 139, 603–622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Gallup, A. C., White, D. D., & Gallup, G. G. (2007). Handgrip strength predicts sexual behavior, body morphology, and aggression in male college students. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 423–429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Gangestad, S. W., & Scheyd, G. J. (2005). The evolution of human physical attractiveness. Annual Review of Anthropology, 34, 523–548.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Gangestad, S. W., & Simpson, J. A. (2000). The evolution of human mating: trade-offs and strategic pluralism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 573–587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., & Garver-Apgar, C. E. (2005). Adaptations to ovulation implications for sexual and social behavior. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 312–316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Gildersleeve, K., Haselton, M. G., & Fales, M. R. (2014). Do women’s mate preferences change across the ovulatory cycle? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 1205–1259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Gladstone, E., & O’Connor, K. M. (2014). A counterpart’s feminine face signals cooperativeness and encourages negotiators to compete. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 125, 18–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Glassenberg, A. N., Feinberg, D. R., Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., & DeBruine, L. M. (2010). Sex-dimorphic face shape preference in heterosexual and homosexual men and women. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 1289–1296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Gustafsson, A., Herrmann, A., & Huber, F. (2007). Conjoint analysis as an instrument of market research practice. In A. Gustafsson, A. Herrmann, & F. Huber (Eds.), Conjoint measurement: methods and applications (pp. 3–30). Heidelberg, Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
  31. Hair, J. F., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L., & Black, W. C. (1995). Multivariate data analysis with readings. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  32. Hancock, P. J., Bruce, V., & Burton, A. M. (2000). Recognition of unfamiliar faces. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 330–337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Hehman, E., Carpinella, C. M., Johnson, K. L., Leitner, J. B., & Freeman, J. B. (2014). Early processing of gendered facial cues predicts the electoral success of female politicians. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5, 815–824.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Jasienska, G., Lipson, S. F., Ellison, P. T., Thune, I., & Ziomkiewicz, A. (2006). Symmetrical women have higher potential fertility. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27, 390–400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Johnston, V. S., & Franklin, M. (1993). Is beauty in the eye of the beholder? Ethology and Sociobiology, 14, 183–199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Johnston, V. S., Hagel, R., Franklin, M., Fink, B., & Grammar, K. (2001). Male facial attractiveness: evidence for hormone-mediated adaptive design. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 251–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Johnston, L., Miles, L., & Macrae, C. N. (2008). Was that a man? Sex identification as a function of menstrual cycle and masculinity. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 1185–1194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2004). When facial attractiveness is only skin deep. Perception, 33, 569–576.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Boothroyd, L., DeBruine, L. M., Feinberg, D. R., Smith, M. L., et al. (2005). Commitment to relationships and preferences for femininity and apparent health in faces are strongest on days of the menstrual cycle when progesterone level is high. Hormones and Behavior, 48, 283–290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Watkins, C. D., Welling, L. L., & DeBruine, L. M. (2011). Reported sexual desire predicts men’s preferences for sexually dimorphic cues in women’s faces. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40, 1281–1285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Keating, C. F., Randall, D. W., Kendrick, T., & Gutshall, K. A. (2003). Do babyfaced adults receive more help? The (cross-cultural) case of the lost resume. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27, 89–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Law Smith, M., Perrett, D. I., Jones, B. C., Cornwell, R. E., Moore, F. R., Feinberg, D. R., et al. (2006). Facial appearance is a cue to oestrogen levels in women. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 273, 135–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Lefevre, C. E., & Perrett, D. I. (2015). Fruit over sunbed: carotenoid skin colouration is found more attractive than melanin colouration. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68, 284–293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Li, N. P. (2007). Mate preference necessities in long- and short-term mating: people prioritize in themselves what their mates prioritize in them. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 39, 528–535.Google Scholar
  45. Li, N. P., Bailey, J. M., Kenrick, D. T., & Linsenmeier, J. A. W. (2002). The necessities and luxuries of mate preferences: testing the tradeoffs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 947–955.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Li, N. P., Valentine, K. A., & Patel, L. (2011). Mate preferences in the U.S. and Singapore: a cross-cultural test of the mate preference priority model. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 291–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Little, A. C., & Jones, B. C. (2012). Variation in facial masculinity and symmetry preferences across the menstrual cycle is moderated by relationship context. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37, 999–1008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Little, A. C., & Mannion, H. (2006). Viewing attractive or unattractive same-sex individuals changes self-rated attractiveness and face preferences in women. Animal Behaviour, 72, 981–987.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., Penton-Voak, I. S., Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2002). Partnership status and the temporal context of relationships influence human female preferences for sexual dimorphism in male face shape. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 269, 1095–1100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Little, A. C., Apicella, C. L., & Marlowe, F. W. (2007a). Preferences for symmetry in human faces in two cultures: data from the UK and the Hadza, an isolated group of hunter-gatherers. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 274, 3113–3117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2007b). Preferences for symmetry in faces change across the menstrual cycle. Biological Psychology, 76, 209–216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., Waitt, C., Tiddeman, B. P., Feinberg, D. R., Perrett, D. I., et al. (2008). Symmetry is related to sexual dimorphism in faces: data across culture and species. PloS One, 3, e2106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Little, A. C., Connely, J., Feinberg, D. R., Jones, B. C., & Roberts, S. C. (2011). Human preference for masculinity differs according to context in faces, bodies, voices, and smell. Behavioral Ecology, 22, 862–868.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Little, A. C., Třebický, V., Havlíček, J., Roberts, S. C., & Kleisner, K. (2015). Human perception of fighting ability: facial cues predict winners and losers in mixed martial arts fights. Behavioral Ecology, 26, 1–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Lohrke, F. T., Holloway, B. B., & Woolley, T. W. (2010). Conjoint analysis in entrepreneurship research a review and research agenda. Organizational Research Methods, 13, 16–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Luce, R. D., & Tukey, J. W. (1964). Simultaneous conjoint measurement: a new type of fundamental measurement. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 1, 1–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Matts, P. J., & Fink, B. (2010). Chronic sun damage and the perception of age, health and attractiveness. Photochemical and Photobiological Sciences, 9, 421–431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Matts, P. J., Fink, B., Grammer, K., & Burquest, M. (2007). Color homogeneity and visual perception of age, health, and attractiveness of female facial skin. Journal of the American Academy of Dermatology, 57, 977–984.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. McKone, E., & Yovel, G. (2009). Why does picture-plane inversion sometimes dissociate perception of features and spacing in faces, and sometimes not? Toward a new theory of holistic processing. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16, 778–797.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Mogilski, J. K., Wade, T. J., & Welling, L. L. M. (2014). Prioritization of potential mates’ history of sexual fidelity during a conjoint ranking task. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 884–897.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Møller, A. P. (1990). Fluctuating asymmetry in male sexual ornaments may reliably reveal male quality. Animal Behaviour, 40, 1185–1187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Møller, A. P. (1997). Developmental stability and fitness: a review. The American Naturalist, 149, 916–932.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Møller, A. P., & Swaddle, J. P. (1997). Asymmetry, developmental stability and evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  64. Møller, A. P., & Thornhill, R. (1997). A meta-analysis of the heritability of developmental stability. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 10, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Moller, A. P., Christe, P., & Lux, E. (1999). Parasitism, host immune function, and sexual selection. Quarterly Review of Biology, 74, 3–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Moore, F. R., Law Smith, M., Taylor, V., & Perrett, D. I. (2011). Sexual dimorphism in the female face is a cue to health and social status but not age. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 1068–1073.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Muhe, L., Oljira, B., Degefu, H., Enquesellassie, F., & Weber, M. W. (1999). Clinical algorithm for malaria during low and high transmission seasons. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 81, 216–220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Muhe, L., Oljira, B., Degefu, H., Jaffar, S., & Weber, M. W. (2000). Evaluation of clinical pallor in the identification and treatment of children with moderate and severe anaemia. Tropical Medicine and International Health, 5, 805–810.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. O’Toole, A. J., Deffenbacher, K. A., Valentin, D., McKee, K., Huff, D., & Abdi, H. (1998). The perception of face gender: the role of stimulus structure in recognition and classification. Memory and Cognition, 26, 146–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Penton-Voak, I. S., & Chen, J. Y. (2004). High salivary testosterone is linked to masculine male facial appearance in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25, 229–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Penton-Voak, I. S., Perrett, D. I., Castles, D. L., Kobayashi, T., Burt, D. M., Murray, L. K., & Minamisawa, R. (1999). Menstrual cycle alters face preference. Nature, 399, 741–742.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Penton-Voak, I. S., Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Baker, S., Tiddeman, B., Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). Symmetry, sexual dimorphism in facial proportions and male facial attractiveness. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 268, 1617–1623.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Penton-Voak, I. S., Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., Burt, D. M., Tiddeman, B. P., & Perrett, D. I. (2003). Female condition influences preferences for sexual dimorphism in faces of male humans (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117, 264–271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Penton-Voak, I. S., Jacobson, A., & Trivers, R. (2004). Populational differences in attractiveness judgements of male and female faces: comparing British and Jamaican samples. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25, 355–370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Perrett, D. I., Lee, K. J., Penton-Voak, I., Rowland, D., Yoshikawa, S., Burt, D. M., et al. (1998). Effects of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness. Nature, 394, 884–887.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Perrett, D. I., Burt, D. M., Penton-Voak, I. S., Lee, K. J., Rowland, D. A., & Edwards, R. (1999). Symmetry and human facial attractiveness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 20, 295–307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Peters, M., Rhodes, G., & Simmons, L. W. (2007). Contributions of the face and body to overall attractiveness. Animal Behaviour, 73, 937–942.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Piepers, D., & Robbins, R. (2012). A review and clarification of the terms “holistic,” “configural,” and “relational” in the face perception literature. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 1–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Puts, D. A. (2010). Beauty and the beast: mechanisms of sexual selection in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31, 157–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Rhodes, G. (2006). The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 199–226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Rhodes, G., Hickford, C., & Jeffery, L. (2000). Sex-typicality and attractiveness: are supermale and superfemale faces super-attractive? British Journal of Psychology, 91, 125–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Rhodes, G., Yoshikawa, S., Clark, A., Lee, K., McKay, R., & Akamatsu, S. (2001). Attractiveness of facial averageness and symmetry in non-western cultures: in search of biologically based standards of beauty. Perception, 30, 611–625.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Rhodes, G., Chan, J., Zebrowitz, L. A., & Simmons, L. W. (2003). Does sexual dimorphism in human faces signal health? Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 270, S93–S95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Rossion, B. (2008). Picture-plane inversion leads to qualitative changes of face perception. Acta Psychologica, 128, 274–289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Rowland, D., & Perrett, D. (1995). Manipulating facial appearance through shape and color. Computer Graphics and Applications, IEEE, 15, 70–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Said, C. P., & Todorov, A. (2011). A statistical model of facial attractiveness. Psychological Science, 22, 1183–1190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Samson, N., Fink, B., & Matts, P. J. (2010). Visible skin condition and perception of human facial appearance. International Journal of Cosmetic Science, 32, 167–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Scheib, J. E. (2001). Context-specific mate choice criteria: Women’s trade-offs in the contexts of long-term and extra-pair mateships. Personal Relationships, 8, 371–389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Schwaninger, A., Wallraven, C., & Bülthoff, H. H. (2004). Computational modeling of face recognition based on psychophysical experiments. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 63, 207–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Scott, I. M., & Penton-Voak, I. S. (2011). The validity of composite photographs for assessing masculinity preferences. Perception, 40, 323–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Scott, I. M., Pound, N., Stephen, I. D., Clark, A. P., & Penton-Voak, I. S. (2010). Does masculinity matter? The contribution of masculine face shape to male attractiveness in humans. PloS One, 5, e13585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Shepherd, D., & Zacharakis, A. (1997). Conjoint analysis: a window of opportunity for entrepreneurship research. In K. Katz & R. Brockhaus (Eds.), Advances in entrepreneurship, firm emergence, and growth (pp. 203–248). Greenwich, CT: JAI.Google Scholar
  93. Smith, F. G., Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., & Little, A. C. (2009). Interactions between masculinity-femininity and apparent health in face preferences. Behavioral Ecology, 20, 441–445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. Stephen, I. D., Coetzee, V., Law Smith, M., & Perrett, D. I. (2009a). Skin blood perfusion and oxygenation colour affect perceived human health. PloS One, 4, 1–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Stephen, I. D., Law Smith, M. J., Stirrat, M. R., & Perrett, D. I. (2009b). Facial skin coloration affects perceived health of human faces. International Journal of Primatology, 30, 845–857.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Stephen, I. D., Coetzee, V., & Perrett, D. I. (2011). Carotenoid and melanin pigment coloration affect perceived human health. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32, 216–227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Stephen, I. D., Scott, I. M., Coetzee, V., Pound, N., Perrett, D. I., & Penton-Voak, I. S. (2012). Cross-cultural effects of color, but not morphological masculinity, on perceived attractiveness of men’s faces. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33, 260–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Symons, D. (1979). The evolution of human sexuality. NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  99. Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (1996). The evolution of human sexuality. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 11, 98–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  100. Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (1999). Facial attractiveness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 452–460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  101. Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (2006). Facial sexual dimorphism, developmental stability, and susceptibility to disease in men and women. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27, 131–144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  102. Thornton, M. J. (2002). The biological actions of estrogens on skin. Experimental Dermatology, 11, 487–502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  103. Tiddeman, B., Burt, M., & Perrett, D. (2001). Prototyping and transforming facial textures for perception research. Computer Graphics and Applications, IEEE, 21, 42–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Vukovic, J., Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Little, A. C., Feinberg, D. R., & Welling, L. L. M. (2009). Circum-menopausal effects on women’s judgements of facial attractiveness. Biology Letters, 5, 62–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  105. Wayneforth, D. (2001). Mate choice trade-offs and women’s preference for physically attractive men. Human Nature, 12, 207–219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  106. Welling, L. L. M., Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Conway, C. A., Smith, M. L., Little, A. C., et al. (2007). Raised salivary testosterone in women is associated with increased attraction to masculine faces. Hormones and Behavior, 52, 156–161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Welling, L. L. M., Jones, B. C., & DeBruine, L. M. (2008a). Sex drive is positively associated with women’s preferences for sexual dimorphism in men’s and women’s faces. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 161–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  108. Welling, L. L. M., Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Smith, F. G., Feinberg, D. R., Little, A. C., & Al-Dujaili, E. A. (2008b). Men report stronger attraction to femininity in women’s faces when their testosterone levels are high. Hormones and Behavior, 54, 703–708.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  109. Whitehead, R. D., Ozakinci, G., Stephen, I. D., & Perrett, D. I. (2012a). Appealing to vanity: could potential appearance improvement motivate fruit and vegetable consumption? American Journal of Public Health, 102, 207–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  110. Whitehead, R. D., Re, D., Xiao, D., Ozakinci, G., & Perrett, D. I. (2012b). You are what you eat: within-subject increases in fruit and vegetable consumption confer beneficial skin-color changes. PloS One, 7, 1–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  111. Wilson, T. D., & Dunn, D. S. (1986). Effects of introspection on attitude-behavior consistency: analyzing reasons versus focusing on feelings. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 22, 249–263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  112. Zaidel, D. W., Aarde, S. M., & Baig, K. (2005). Appearance of symmetry, beauty, and health in human faces. Brain and Cognition, 57, 261–263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  113. Zilioli, S., Sell, A. N., Stirrat, M., Jagore, J., Vickerman, W., & Watson, N. V. (2014). Face of a fighter: bizygomatic width as a cue of formidability. Aggressive Behavior, 41, 322–330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyOakland UniversityRochesterUSA

Personalised recommendations