Abstract
Cognitive scientists have increasingly turned to cultural transmission to explain the widespread nature of religion. One key hypothesis focuses on memory, proposing that that minimally counterintuitive (MCI) content facilitates the transmission of supernatural beliefs. We propose two caveats to this hypothesis. (1) Memory effects decrease as MCI concepts become commonly used, and (2) people do not believe counterintuitive content readily; therefore additional mechanisms are required to get from memory to belief. In experiments 1–3 (n = 283), we examined the relationship between MCI, belief, and memory. We found that increased tendencies to anthropomorphize predicted poorer memory for anthropomorphic-MCI content. MCI content was found less believable than intuitive content, suggesting different mechanisms are required to explain belief. In experiment 4 (n = 70), we examined the non-content-based cultural learning mechanism of credibility-enhancing displays (CREDs) and found that it increased participants’ belief in MCI content, suggesting this type of learning can better explain the transmission of belief.
Similar content being viewed by others
Notes
Prodigies are anecdotes or microstories about transgressions in the natural order of the cosmos. Prodigies were reported to the Senate, who decided which were genuine and should be recorded.
Since each story could be intuitive, anthropomorphic-MCI, or physical-MCI, and story type was randomized, the total number of concepts did not differ across content type even though it did differ by story.
Using a multilevel logistic regression with random intercepts for each individual we can show that these effects do not change when we control for age, gender, ethnicity, and religious affiliation. MCI items are significantly more memorable than intuitive items after 5 min (z = 4.98, p < 0.001; Odds: 1.77, 95%CI: 1.41 to 2.21) and one week (z = 3.51, p < 0.001; Odds: 1.66, 95%CI: 1.25 to 2.21). Belief was similarly modeled. Belief changed an average of −3.08 (95 % CI −3.39 to −2.77; t 81.00 = −19.46, p < 0.001, d = −9.26) points on a seven-point scale between intuitive items and MCI items after 5 min, and −3.46 (95%CI: −3.60 to −3.32; t 165.40 = −49.53, p < 0.001) after one week.
Using a multilevel model with random intercepts for each individual, belief is associated with a change of −1.59 (95%CI: −1.67 to −1.39; t 105.29 = −15.65; p < 0.001) points on a seven-point scale when comparing MCI and intuitive stories after 5 min, and an average of −1.58 (95%CI: −1.85 to −1.32; t 118.70 = 11.77, p < 0.001) points on seven-point scale after 1 week.
References
Atran, S. (2002). In gods we trust: The evolutionary landscape of religion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Atran, S., & Henrich, J. (2010). The evolution of religion: how cognitive by-products, adaptive learning heuristics, ritual displays, and group competition generate deep commitments to prosocial religions. Biological Theory, 5(1), 18–30.
Atran, S., & Norenzayan, A. (2004a). Religion’s evolutionary landscape: counterintuition, committment, compassion, communion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 713–730.
Atran, S., & Norenzayan, A. (2004b). Why minds create gods: devotion, deception, death, and arational decision making. [reply to commentators]. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 754–770.
Barrett, J. L. (2004). Why would anyone believe in God? Walnut Creek: AltaMira Press.
Barrett, J. L. (2008a). Coding and quantifying counterintuitiveness in religious concepts: theoretical and methodological reflections. Method & Theory in the Study of Religion, 20(4), 308–338.
Barrett, J. L. (2008b). Why Santa Claus is not a god. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 8(1), 149–161.
Barrett, J. L., & Nyhof, M. A. (2001). Spreading nonnatural concepts. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 1, 69–100.
Bergstrom, B., Moehlmann, B., & Boyer, P. (2006). Extending the testimony problem: evaluating the truth, scope, and source of cultural information. Child Development, 77(3), 531–538.
Boyer, P. (1994). The naturalness of religious ideas: A cognitive theory of religion. Berkley: University of California Press.
Boyer, P. (2001). Religion explained: The evolutionary origins of religious thought. New York: Basic Books.
Boyer, P. (2003). Religious thought and behaviour as by-products of brain function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 119–124.
Boyer, P., & Ramble, C. (2001). Cognitive templates for religious concepts: cross-cultural evidence for recall of counter-intuitive representations. Cognitive Science, 25, 535–564.
Brewer, W. F. (1985). The story schema: universal and culture-specific properties. In D. R. Olson, N. Torrance, & A. Hildyard (Eds.), Literacy, language, and learning (pp. 167–194). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Cohen, A. B., Siegel, J. I., & Rozin, P. (2003). Faith versus practice: different bases for religiosity judgments by Jews and Protestants. European Journal of Social Psychology, 33(2), 287–295.
Epley, N., Akalis, S., Waytz, A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2008a). Creating social connection through inferential reproduction: loneliness and perceived agency in gadgets, gods, and greyhounds. Psychological Science, 19, 114–120.
Epley, N., Waytz, A., Akalis, S., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2008b). When we need a human: motivational determinants of anthropomorphism. Social Cognition, 26, 143–155.
Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102(2), 211–245.
Gervais, W. M., & Henrich, J. (2010). The Zeus problem: why representational content biases cannot explain faith in gods. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 10, 383–389.
Gervais, W. M., Willard, A. K., Norenzayan, A., & Henrich, J. (2011). The cultural transmission of faith: why natural intuitions and memory biases are necessary, but insufficient, to explain religious belief. Religion, 41(1), 389–400.
Gonce, L. O., Upal, M. A., Slone, D. J., & Tweney, D. R. (2006). Role of context in the recall of counterintuitive concepts. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 6, 521–547.
Greenhouse, S. W., & Geisser, S. (1959). On methods in the analysis of profile data. Psychometrika, 24, 95–112.
Harris, P. L., & Koenig, M. A. (2006). Trust in testimony: how children learn about science and religion. Child Development, 77(3), 505–524.
Henrich, J. (2009). The evolution of costly displays, cooperation, and religion: credibility enhancing displays and their implications for cultural evolution. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30(244–260), 244–260.
Henrich, J., & Boyd, R. (1998). The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence of between-group differences. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19, 215–241.
Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige: freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22(3), 165–196.
Henrich, J., & McElreath, R. (2003). The evolution of cultural evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology, 12(12), 123–135.
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 61–83.
Lang, V. A. (1995). Relative association, interactiveness, and the bizarre imagery effect. The American Journal of Psychology, 108(1), 13–35.
Lisdorf, A. (2004). The spread of non-natural concepts. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 4, 151–173.
McDaniel, M. A., Einstein, G. O., DeLosh, E. L., May, C. P., & Brady, P. (1995). The bizarreness effect: it’s not surprising, it’s complex. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(2), 422–435.
Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., & Dunbar, R. (2006). A bias for social information in human cultural transmission. British Journal of Psychology, 97, 405–423.
Norenzayan, A. (2013). Big gods: How religion transformed cooperation and conflict. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Norenzayan, A., Atran, S., Faulkner, J., & Schaller, M. (2006). Memory and mystery: the cultural selection of minimally counterintuitive narratives. Cognitive Science, 30(3), 531–553.
Purzycki, B. G. (2010). Cognitive architecture, humor and counterintuitiveness: retention and recall of MCIs. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 10, 189–204.
Purzycki, B. G., & Sosis, R. (2011). Our gods: variation in supernatural minds. In U. J. Frey, C. Störmer, & K. P. Willführ (Eds.), Essential building blocks of human nature (pp. 77–93). Berlin: Springer.
Purzycki, B. G., & Willard, A. K. (2015). MCI theory: a critical discussion. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 1–42.
Pyysiäinen, I., Lindeman, M., & Honkela, T. (2003). Counterintuitiveness as the hallmark of religiosity. Religion, 33(4), 341–355.
Riefer, D., & Lamay, M. (1998). Memory for common and bizarre stimuli: a storage-retrieval analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5(2), 312–317.
Rubin, D. C. (1995). Memory in oral traditions: The cognitive psychology of epic, ballads, and counting-out rhymes. New York: Oxford University Press.
Russell, Y. I., & Gobet, F. (2013). What is counterintuitive? Religious cognition and natural expectation. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 4(4), 715–749.
Shariff, A. F., Purzycki, B. G., & Sosis, R. (2014). Religions as cultural solutions to social living. In A. B. Cohen (Ed.), Culture reexamined (pp. 217–238). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Spelke, E. S., & Kinzler, K. D. (2007). Core knowledge. Developmental Science, 10(1), 89–96.
Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining culture: A naturalistic approach. Oxford: Blackwell.
Traub, R. E., & Rowley, G. L. (1991). Understanding reliability. ITEMS, 10, 37–45.
Upal, M. A. (2010). An alternative account of the minimal counterintuitiveness effect. Cognitive Systems Research, 11(2), 194–203.
Upal, M. A., Gonce, L. O., Tweney, R. D., & Slone, D. J. (2007). Contextualizing counterintuitiveness: how context affects comprehension and memorability of counterintuitive concepts. Cognitive Science, 31(3), 415–439.
Waytz, A., Cacioppo, J., & Epley, N. (2010a). Who sees human? The stability and importance of individual differences in anthropomorphism. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5(3), 219–232.
Waytz, A., Morewedge, C. K., Epley, N., Monteleone, G., Gao, J.-H., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2010b). Making sense by making sentient: effectance motivation increases anthropomorphism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(3), 410–435.
Wellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. (1992). Cognitive development: foundational theories of core domains. Annual Review of Psychology, 43(1), 337–375.
Willard, A. K., & Norenzayan, A. (2013). Cognitive biases explain religious belief, paranormal belief, and belief in life’s purpose. Cognition, 129(2), 379–391.
Zaragoza, M. S., & Mitchell, K. J. (1996). Repeated exposure to suggestion and the creation of false memories. Psychological Science, 7(5), 294–300.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank Benjamin Purzycki and Adam Baimel for their helpful comments. JH would like to thank the Canadian Institute for Advanced Research and NYU’s Stern School of Business. AN acknowledges support from a Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council (SSHRC) Insight Grant (435-2014-0456). We also thank the Cultural Evolution of Religion Research Consortium, funded by a generous partnership grant (895-2011-1009) also from SSHRC.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Electronic supplementary material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
ESM 1
(PDF 67.1 kb)
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Willard, A.K., Henrich, J. & Norenzayan, A. Memory and Belief in the Transmission of Counterintuitive Content. Hum Nat 27, 221–243 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-016-9259-6
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-016-9259-6