Abstract
It has previously been suggested that the historically and geographically widespread persistence of religious beliefs occurs because it is a by-product of normal cognitive processes, ones which first evolved to confer survival advantages in the social domain. If this theory holds, then it is likely that inter-individual variation in the same biases may predict corresponding variation in religious thoughts and behaviors. Using an online questionnaire, 298 participants answered questions regarding their tendency to detect agency, the degree to which they displayed schizotypal traits, their ability to understand the emotions and motivations of others (“mentalizing”), and their religious beliefs and behaviors. Path analysis suggests that mentalizing, agency detection, and schizotypal thinking were each independently related to religiosity. Furthermore, schizotypal thinking and agency detection were highly interrelated with one another, whereas mentalizing was not. Although the degree to which an individual engages with religious or spiritual beliefs will be influenced by their cultural and historical context, this paper helps to elucidate the interplay between various cognitive processes that might predispose some individuals but not others toward holding such beliefs in the first place.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Apperly, I. A. (2012). What is “theory of mind”? concepts, cognitive processes and individual differences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(5), 825–839.
Atran, S., & Norenzayan, A. (2005). Religion’s evolutionary landscape: counterintuition, commitment, compassion, communion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(06), 1–58.
Baron-Cohen, S. (2009). Autism: the empathizing-systemizing (E-S) theory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156, 68–80.
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 37–46.
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “Reading the mind in the eyes” test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(2), 241–251.
Barrett, J. L. (2000). Exploring the natural foundations of religion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(1), 29–34.
Barrett, J. L. (2007). Cognitive science of religion: what is it and why is it? Religion Compass, 1(6), 768–786.
Barrett, J. L., & Keil, F. C. (1996). Conceptualizing a nonnatural entity: anthropomorphism in god concepts. Cognitive Psychology, 31(3), 219–247.
Barrett, D. B., Kurian, G. T., & Johnson, T. M. (1998). World Christian encyclopedia. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Bentall, R. P., Claridge, G., & Slade, P. D. (1989). The multidimensional nature of schizotypal traits: a factor analytic study with normal subjects. The British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 28(Pt 4), 363–375.
Bering, J. M. (2002). IntuitivecConceptions of dead agents’ minds: the natural foundations of afterlife beliefs as phenomenological boundary. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 2(4), 263–308.
Boyer, P. (1994). The naturalness of religious ideas: A cognitive theory of religion. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Boyer, P. (2008). Being human: religion: bound to believe? Nature, 455(7216), 1038–1039.
Caldwell-Harris, C.L., Murphy, C.F., Velazquez, T., McNamara, P. (2011). Religious belief systems of persons with high functioning autism. Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Austin, pp. 3362–3366. Available from: http://csjarchive.cogsci.rpi.edu/proceedings/2011/papers/0782/paper0782.pdf.
Cartmill, E. A., & Byrne, R. W. (2007). Orangutans modify their gestural signaling according to their audience’s comprehension. Current Biology, 17(15), 1345–1348.
Dall, S. R. X., Houston, A. I., & McNamara, J. M. (2004). The behavioural ecology of personality: consistent individual differences from an adaptive perspective. Ecology Letters. doi:10.1111/j.1461-0248.2004.00618.x.
Dein, S., & Littlewood, R. (2011). Religion and psychosis: A common evolutionary trajectory? Transcultural Psychiatry, 48(3), 318–335.
Diduca, D., & Joseph, S. (1997). Schizotypal traits and dimensions of religiosity. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36(4), 635–638.
Dunbar, R. I. M. (2003). The social brain: mind, language, and society in evolutionary perspective. Annual Review of Anthropology, 32(1), 163–181.
Dunbar, R. I. M. (2008). Mind the gap; or why humans are not just Great Apes. Proceedings of the British Academy, 154, 403–423.
Eisenberg, N., & Lennon, R. (1984). Sex differences in empathy and related capacities. Psychological Bulletin, 94(1), 100–131.
Epley, N., Waytz, A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2007). On seeing human: A three-factor theory of anthropomorphism. Psychological Review, 114(4), 864–886.
Farias, M., Claridge, G., & Lalljee, M. (2005). Personality and cognitive predictors of New Age practices and beliefs. Personality and Individual Differences, 39(5), 979–989.
Frith, C. D. (1992). The cognitive neuropsychology of schizophrenia. Hove: LEA.
Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences, 358(1431), 459–473.
Guthrie, S. (1980). A cognitive theory of religion. Current Anthropology, 21(2), 181–203.
Guthrie, S. (1993). Faces in the clouds. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., & Layton, J. B. (2010). Social relationships and mortality risk: A meta-analytic review. PLoS Medicine, 7(7), e1000316.
Kapogiannis, D., Barbey, A. K., Su, M., Zamboni, G., Krueger, F., & Grafman, J. (2009). Cognitive and neural foundations of religious belief. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(12), 4876–4881.
Koenig, H. G., & Büssing, A. (2010). The Duke University religion index (DUREL): A five-item measure for use in epidemological studies. Religion, 1(1), 78–85.
Korkmaz, B. (2011). Theory of mind and neurodevelopmental disorders of childhood. Pediatric Research, 69(5 Pt 2), 101R–108R.
Lane, J. D., Wellman, H. M., & Evans, E. M. (2010). Children’ s understanding of ordinary and extraordinary minds. Child Development, 81(5), 1475–1489.
Launay, J., Pearce, E., Wlodarski, R., van Duijn, M., Carney, J., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2015). Higher-order mentalising and executive functioning. Personality and Individual Differences, 86, 6–14.
Loewen, P. J., Lyle, G., & Nachshen, J. S. (2010). An eight-item form of the Empathy Quotient (EQ) and an application to charitable giving. Retrieved from www.crcee.umontreal.ca/pdf/EightQuestionES.
Maltby, J., & Day, L. (2002). Religious experience, religious orientation and schizotypy. Mental Health, Religion and Culture, 5(2), 163–174.
Maynard Smith, J. (1984). Game theory and the evolution of behaviour. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7(1), 95–125.
McCauley, R. N. (2000). The naturalness of religion and the unnaturalness of science. In F. C. Keil & R. Wilson (Eds.), Explanation and cognition (pp. 61–85). Cambridge: MIT Press.
Michotte, A. (1963). The perception of causality. (T. R. Miles & E. Miles, Eds.). New York: Basic Books.
Norenzayan, A. (2013). Big gods: How religion transformed cooperation and conflict. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Norenzayan, A., Gervais, W. M., & Trzesniewski, K. H. (2012). Mentalizing deficits constrain belief in a personal god. PloS One, 7(5), e36880.
O’Connell, S., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2003). A test for comprehension of false belief in chimpanzees. Evolution and Cognition, 9(2), 131–140.
Pearce, E. (2014). Modelling mechanisms of social network maintenance in hunter-gatherers. Journal of Archaeological Science, 50, 403–413.
Peters, E., Day, S., McKenna, J., & Orbach, G. (1999). Delusional ideation in religious and psychotic populations. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 38(1), 83–96.
Plante, T. G., Vallaeys, C. L., Sherman, A. C., & Wallston, K. A. (2002). The development of a brief version of the Santa Clara Strength of Religious Faith Questionnaire. Pastoral Psychology, 50(5), 359–368.
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 515–526.
Previc, F. H. (2006). The role of the extrapersonal brain systems in religious activity. Consciousness and Cognition, 15(3), 500–539.
Raine, A. (1991). The SPQ: a scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 17(4), 555–564.
Riekki, T., Lindeman, M., Aleneff, M., Halme, A., & Nuortimo, A. (2013). Paranormal and religious believers are more prone to illusory face perception than skeptics and nonbelievers. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 27(2), 150–155.
Saucier, G., & Skrzypińska, K. (2006). Spiritual but not religious? evidence for two independent dispositions. Journal of Personality, 74(5), 1257–1292.
Schjoedt, U., Stødkilde-Jørgensen, H., Geertz, A. W., & Roepstorff, A. (2009). Highly religious participants recruit areas of social cognition in personal prayer. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4(2), 199–207.
Shtulman, A. (2008). Variation in the anthropomorphization of supernatural beings and its implications for cognitive theories of religion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(5), 1123–1138.
Stiller, J., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2007). Perspective-taking and memory capacity predict social network size. Social Networks, 29(1), 93–104.
von Oertzen, T., Brandmaier, A. M., & Tsang, S. (2014). Structural equation modeling with Ωnyx. Structural Equation Modeling, 22(1), 148–161.
Wakabayashi, A., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Goldenfeld, N., Delaney, J., Fine, D., & Weil, L. (2006). Development of short forms of the empathy quotient (EQ-short) and the systemizing quotient (SQ-short). Personality and Individual Differences, 41(5), 929–940.
Walter, T., & Davie, G. (1998). The religiosity of women in the modern West. The British Journal of Sociology, 49(4), 640–660.
Waytz, A., Cacioppo, J. T., & Epley, N. (2010). Who sees human? The stability and importance of individual differences in anthropomorphism. Psychological Science, 5(3), 219–232.
Willard, A. K., & Norenzayan, A. (2013). Cognitive biases explain religious belief, paranormal belief, and belief in life’s purpose. Cognition, 129(2), 379–391.
Wlodarski, R., Manning, J. T., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2015). Stay or stray? Evidence for alternative mating strategy phenotypes in both men and women. Biology Letters, 11(2), 20140977.
Acknowledgments
We thank our colleagues at the Social and Evolutionary Neuroscience Research Group (SENRG) for their valuable input, particularly Jacques Launay and Robin I. M. Dunbar. RW and EP are supported by a European Research Council Advanced Grant.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Wlodarski, R., Pearce, E. The God Allusion. Hum Nat 27, 160–172 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-016-9256-9
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-016-9256-9