Skip to main content
Log in

The God Allusion

Individual Variation in Agency Detection, Mentalizing and Schizotypy and Their Association with Religious Beliefs and Behaviors

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

It has previously been suggested that the historically and geographically widespread persistence of religious beliefs occurs because it is a by-product of normal cognitive processes, ones which first evolved to confer survival advantages in the social domain. If this theory holds, then it is likely that inter-individual variation in the same biases may predict corresponding variation in religious thoughts and behaviors. Using an online questionnaire, 298 participants answered questions regarding their tendency to detect agency, the degree to which they displayed schizotypal traits, their ability to understand the emotions and motivations of others (“mentalizing”), and their religious beliefs and behaviors. Path analysis suggests that mentalizing, agency detection, and schizotypal thinking were each independently related to religiosity. Furthermore, schizotypal thinking and agency detection were highly interrelated with one another, whereas mentalizing was not. Although the degree to which an individual engages with religious or spiritual beliefs will be influenced by their cultural and historical context, this paper helps to elucidate the interplay between various cognitive processes that might predispose some individuals but not others toward holding such beliefs in the first place.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Apperly, I. A. (2012). What is “theory of mind”? concepts, cognitive processes and individual differences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(5), 825–839.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atran, S., & Norenzayan, A. (2005). Religion’s evolutionary landscape: counterintuition, commitment, compassion, communion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(06), 1–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2009). Autism: the empathizing-systemizing (E-S) theory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156, 68–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 37–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “Reading the mind in the eyes” test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(2), 241–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, J. L. (2000). Exploring the natural foundations of religion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(1), 29–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, J. L. (2007). Cognitive science of religion: what is it and why is it? Religion Compass, 1(6), 768–786.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, J. L., & Keil, F. C. (1996). Conceptualizing a nonnatural entity: anthropomorphism in god concepts. Cognitive Psychology, 31(3), 219–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, D. B., Kurian, G. T., & Johnson, T. M. (1998). World Christian encyclopedia. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Bentall, R. P., Claridge, G., & Slade, P. D. (1989). The multidimensional nature of schizotypal traits: a factor analytic study with normal subjects. The British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 28(Pt 4), 363–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bering, J. M. (2002). IntuitivecConceptions of dead agents’ minds: the natural foundations of afterlife beliefs as phenomenological boundary. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 2(4), 263–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, P. (1994). The naturalness of religious ideas: A cognitive theory of religion. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, P. (2008). Being human: religion: bound to believe? Nature, 455(7216), 1038–1039.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caldwell-Harris, C.L., Murphy, C.F., Velazquez, T., McNamara, P. (2011). Religious belief systems of persons with high functioning autism. Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Austin, pp. 3362–3366. Available from: http://csjarchive.cogsci.rpi.edu/proceedings/2011/papers/0782/paper0782.pdf.

  • Cartmill, E. A., & Byrne, R. W. (2007). Orangutans modify their gestural signaling according to their audience’s comprehension. Current Biology, 17(15), 1345–1348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dall, S. R. X., Houston, A. I., & McNamara, J. M. (2004). The behavioural ecology of personality: consistent individual differences from an adaptive perspective. Ecology Letters. doi:10.1111/j.1461-0248.2004.00618.x.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dein, S., & Littlewood, R. (2011). Religion and psychosis: A common evolutionary trajectory? Transcultural Psychiatry, 48(3), 318–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diduca, D., & Joseph, S. (1997). Schizotypal traits and dimensions of religiosity. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36(4), 635–638.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (2003). The social brain: mind, language, and society in evolutionary perspective. Annual Review of Anthropology, 32(1), 163–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (2008). Mind the gap; or why humans are not just Great Apes. Proceedings of the British Academy, 154, 403–423.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, N., & Lennon, R. (1984). Sex differences in empathy and related capacities. Psychological Bulletin, 94(1), 100–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epley, N., Waytz, A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2007). On seeing human: A three-factor theory of anthropomorphism. Psychological Review, 114(4), 864–886.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farias, M., Claridge, G., & Lalljee, M. (2005). Personality and cognitive predictors of New Age practices and beliefs. Personality and Individual Differences, 39(5), 979–989.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, C. D. (1992). The cognitive neuropsychology of schizophrenia. Hove: LEA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences, 358(1431), 459–473.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, S. (1980). A cognitive theory of religion. Current Anthropology, 21(2), 181–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, S. (1993). Faces in the clouds. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., & Layton, J. B. (2010). Social relationships and mortality risk: A meta-analytic review. PLoS Medicine, 7(7), e1000316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kapogiannis, D., Barbey, A. K., Su, M., Zamboni, G., Krueger, F., & Grafman, J. (2009). Cognitive and neural foundations of religious belief. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(12), 4876–4881.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koenig, H. G., & Büssing, A. (2010). The Duke University religion index (DUREL): A five-item measure for use in epidemological studies. Religion, 1(1), 78–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Korkmaz, B. (2011). Theory of mind and neurodevelopmental disorders of childhood. Pediatric Research, 69(5 Pt 2), 101R–108R.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lane, J. D., Wellman, H. M., & Evans, E. M. (2010). Children’ s understanding of ordinary and extraordinary minds. Child Development, 81(5), 1475–1489.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Launay, J., Pearce, E., Wlodarski, R., van Duijn, M., Carney, J., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2015). Higher-order mentalising and executive functioning. Personality and Individual Differences, 86, 6–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loewen, P. J., Lyle, G., & Nachshen, J. S. (2010). An eight-item form of the Empathy Quotient (EQ) and an application to charitable giving. Retrieved from www.crcee.umontreal.ca/pdf/EightQuestionES.

  • Maltby, J., & Day, L. (2002). Religious experience, religious orientation and schizotypy. Mental Health, Religion and Culture, 5(2), 163–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith, J. (1984). Game theory and the evolution of behaviour. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7(1), 95–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCauley, R. N. (2000). The naturalness of religion and the unnaturalness of science. In F. C. Keil & R. Wilson (Eds.), Explanation and cognition (pp. 61–85). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michotte, A. (1963). The perception of causality. (T. R. Miles & E. Miles, Eds.). New York: Basic Books.

  • Norenzayan, A. (2013). Big gods: How religion transformed cooperation and conflict. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

  • Norenzayan, A., Gervais, W. M., & Trzesniewski, K. H. (2012). Mentalizing deficits constrain belief in a personal god. PloS One, 7(5), e36880.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Connell, S., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2003). A test for comprehension of false belief in chimpanzees. Evolution and Cognition, 9(2), 131–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pearce, E. (2014). Modelling mechanisms of social network maintenance in hunter-gatherers. Journal of Archaeological Science, 50, 403–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peters, E., Day, S., McKenna, J., & Orbach, G. (1999). Delusional ideation in religious and psychotic populations. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 38(1), 83–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plante, T. G., Vallaeys, C. L., Sherman, A. C., & Wallston, K. A. (2002). The development of a brief version of the Santa Clara Strength of Religious Faith Questionnaire. Pastoral Psychology, 50(5), 359–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 515–526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Previc, F. H. (2006). The role of the extrapersonal brain systems in religious activity. Consciousness and Cognition, 15(3), 500–539.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raine, A. (1991). The SPQ: a scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 17(4), 555–564.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riekki, T., Lindeman, M., Aleneff, M., Halme, A., & Nuortimo, A. (2013). Paranormal and religious believers are more prone to illusory face perception than skeptics and nonbelievers. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 27(2), 150–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saucier, G., & Skrzypińska, K. (2006). Spiritual but not religious? evidence for two independent dispositions. Journal of Personality, 74(5), 1257–1292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schjoedt, U., Stødkilde-Jørgensen, H., Geertz, A. W., & Roepstorff, A. (2009). Highly religious participants recruit areas of social cognition in personal prayer. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4(2), 199–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shtulman, A. (2008). Variation in the anthropomorphization of supernatural beings and its implications for cognitive theories of religion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(5), 1123–1138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiller, J., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2007). Perspective-taking and memory capacity predict social network size. Social Networks, 29(1), 93–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • von Oertzen, T., Brandmaier, A. M., & Tsang, S. (2014). Structural equation modeling with Ωnyx. Structural Equation Modeling, 22(1), 148–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wakabayashi, A., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Goldenfeld, N., Delaney, J., Fine, D., & Weil, L. (2006). Development of short forms of the empathy quotient (EQ-short) and the systemizing quotient (SQ-short). Personality and Individual Differences, 41(5), 929–940.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walter, T., & Davie, G. (1998). The religiosity of women in the modern West. The British Journal of Sociology, 49(4), 640–660.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waytz, A., Cacioppo, J. T., & Epley, N. (2010). Who sees human? The stability and importance of individual differences in anthropomorphism. Psychological Science, 5(3), 219–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willard, A. K., & Norenzayan, A. (2013). Cognitive biases explain religious belief, paranormal belief, and belief in life’s purpose. Cognition, 129(2), 379–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wlodarski, R., Manning, J. T., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2015). Stay or stray? Evidence for alternative mating strategy phenotypes in both men and women. Biology Letters, 11(2), 20140977.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank our colleagues at the Social and Evolutionary Neuroscience Research Group (SENRG) for their valuable input, particularly Jacques Launay and Robin I. M. Dunbar. RW and EP are supported by a European Research Council Advanced Grant.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rafael Wlodarski.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wlodarski, R., Pearce, E. The God Allusion. Hum Nat 27, 160–172 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-016-9256-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-016-9256-9

Keywords

Navigation