Skip to main content
Log in

Sex Differences in Exploration Behavior and the Relationship to Harm Avoidance

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Venturing into novel terrain poses physical risks to a female and her offspring. Females have a greater tendency to avoid physical harm, while males tend to have larger range sizes and often outperform females in navigation-related tasks. Given this backdrop, we expected that females would explore a novel environment with more caution than males, and that more-cautious exploration would negatively affect navigation performance. Participants explored a novel, large-scale, virtual environment in search of five objects, pointed in the direction of each object from the origin, and then navigated back to the objects. We found that females demonstrated more caution while exploring as reflected in the increased amounts of pausing and revisiting of previously traversed locations. In addition, more pausing and revisiting behaviors led to degradation in navigation performance. Finally, individual levels of trait harm avoidance were positively associated with the amount of revisiting behavior during exploration. These findings support the idea that the fitness costs associated with long-distance travel may encourage females to take a more cautious approach to spatial exploration, and that this caution may partially explain the sex differences in navigation performance.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The Fréchet distance measures the similarity between two curves by determining the minimum line length required to connect two units as they move along each curve. For example, imagine a dog owner and a dog going for a walk and taking different paths, but staying near each other to some degree. The Fréchet distance simply determines the minimum length of a leash necessary to connect the owner to the dog as they travel through their trajectory. The discrete version of the Fréchet distance is conceptually similar, but instead imagine two frogs that jump from one stone to another, creating a discrete trajectory. The discrete Fréchet distance measures the minimum line length required to connect the frogs at each stone along their trajectory.

References

  • Astur, R. S., Tropp, J., Sava, S., Constable, R. T., & Markus, E. J. (2004). Sex differences and correlations in a virtual Morris water task, a virtual radial arm maze, and mental rotation. Behavioural Brain Research, 151(1), 103–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartumeus, F. (2007). Lévy processes in animal movement: an evolutionary hypothesis. Fractals, 15(2), 1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, D., Ramos-Fernándex, G., Miramontes, O., Mateos, J. L., Cocho, G., Larralde, H., Ramos, H., & Rojas, F. (2006). Scale-free foraging by primates emerges from their interaction with a complex environment. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 273, 1743–1750.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrnes, J. P., Miller, D. C., & Schafer, W. D. (1999). Gender differences in risk taking: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125(3), 367–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, A. (1999). Staying alive: evolution, culture, and women’s intrasexual aggression. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 203–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clauset, A., Shalizi, C. R., & Newman, M. E. (2009). Power-law distributions in empirical data. SIAM Review, 51(4), 661–703.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coluccia, E., & Louse, G. (2004). Gender differences in spatial orientation: a review. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 24, 329–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Croson, R., & Gneezy, U. (2009). Gender differences in preferences. Journal of Economic Literature, 47(2), 448–474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cross, C. P., Cyrenne, D.-L. M., & Brown, G. R. (2013). Sex differences in sensation-seeking: a meta-analysis. Scientific Reports, 3(2486), 1–5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Devlin, A. S., & Bernstein, J. (1995). Interactive wayfinding: use of cues by men and women. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 15(1), 23–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ecuyer-Dab, I., & Robert, M. (2004). Have sex differences in spatial ability evolved from male competition for mating and female concern for survival? Cognition, 91(3), 221–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaulin, S. J. C., & Fitzgerald, R. W. (1989). Sexual selection for spatial-learning ability. Animal Behaviour, 37, 322–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gillespie, C. S. (2015). Fitting heavy tailed distributions: the poweRlaw package. Journal of Statistical Software, 64(2), 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, C. R., Jenkins, M., & Glaser, D. (2006). Gender differences in risk assessment: why do women take fewer risks than men? Judgment and Decision Making, 1(1), 48–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, R. (1979). Children’s experience of place. New York: Irvington.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jasarevic, E., Williams, S. A., Roberts, R. M., Gearly, D. C., & Rosenfeld, C. S. (2012). Spatial navigation strategies in Peromyscus: a comparative study. Animal Behaviour, 84(5), 1141–1149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lawton, C. A. (1994). Gender differences in way-finding strategies: relationship to spatial ability and spatial anxiety. Sex Roles, 30(11–12), 765–779.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lawton, C. A., & Kallai, J. (2002). Gender differences in wayfinding strategies and anxiety about wayfinding: a cross-cultural comparison. Sex Roles, 47(9), 389–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, D. H., & Hewlett, B. S. (1999). Reproductive interests and forager mobility 1. Current Anthropology, 40(4), 501–524.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marks, I. M. (1987). Fears, phobias, and rituals: Panic, anxiety, and their disorders. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, M. H. (1987). Gender, home range and environmental cognition. Transactions of the Institute of British Geographers, 12(1), 43–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miner, E. J., Gurven, M., Kaplan, H., & Gaulin, S. J. (2014). Sex difference in travel is concentrated in adolescence and tracks reproductive interests. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281(1796), 1476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moffat, S. D., Hampson, E., & Hatzipantelis, M. (1998). Navigation in a “virtual” maze: sex differences and correlation with psychometric measures of spatial ability in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19(2), 73–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Montello, D. R., Lovelace, K. L., Golledge, R. G., & Self, C. M. (1999). Sex-related differences and similarities in geographic and environmental spatial abilities. Annals of the Association of American Geographers, 89(3), 515–534.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Munroe, R. L., & Munroe, R. H. (1971). Effect of environmental experience on spatial ability in an East African society. The Journal of Social Psychology, 83(1), 15–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muthén, L., & Muthén, B. (1998–2012). Mplus user’s guide, seventh edition. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén. https://www.statmodel.com/download/usersguide/Mplus%20user%20guide%20Ver_7_r3_web.pdf.

  • Picucci, L., Caffo, A. O., & Bosco, A. (2011). Besides navigation accuracy: gender differences in strategy selection and level of spatial confidence. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 31(4), 430–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raichlen, D. A., Wood, B. M., Gordon, A. D., Mabulla, A. Z. P., Marlowe, F. W., & Pontzer, H. (2014). Evidence of Levy walk foraging patterns in human hunter-gatherers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(2), 728–733.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhee, I., Shin, M., Hong, S., Lee, K., Kim, S. J., & Chong, S. (2011). On the Levy-walk nature of human mobility. IEEE/ACM Transactions on Networking (TON), 19(3), 630–643.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richardson, A. E., Montello, D. R., & Hegarty, M. (1999). Spatial knowledge acquisition from maps and from navigation in real and virtual environments. Memory and Cognition, 27, 741–750.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitz, S. (1997). Gender-related strategies in environmental development: Effects of anxiety on wayfinding in and representation of a three-dimensional maze. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 17(3), 215–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitz, S. (1999). Gender differences in acquisition of environmental knowledge related to wayfinding behavior, spatial anxiety and self-estimated environmental competencies. Sex Roles, 41, 71–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sear, R., & Mace, R. (2008). Who keeps children alive? A review of the effects of kin on child survival. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(1), 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shelton, A. L., & McNamara, T. P. (2001). Systems of spatial reference in human memory. Cognitive Psychology, 43(4), 274–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tellegen, A., & Waller, N. G. (2008). Exploring personality through test construction: development of the multidimensional personality questionnaire. The Sage Handbook of Personality Theory and Assessment, 2, 261–292.

    Google Scholar 

  • Unity (4.6.0) (2014). San Francisco, CA: Unity. Retrieved from https://unity3d.com/unity/whats-new/unity-4.6.

  • Vashro, L., & Cashdan, E. (2015). Spatial cognition, mobility, and reproductive success in northwestern Namibia. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(2), 123–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whiting, B. B., & Edwards, C. P. (1992). Children of different worlds: The formation of social behavior. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by NSF Grant IBSS 1329091

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kyle T. Gagnon.

Additional information

An earlier version of this article won a poster award at the Human Behavior and Evolution Society meetings in Natal, Brazil, 2014.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gagnon, K.T., Cashdan, E.A., Stefanucci, J.K. et al. Sex Differences in Exploration Behavior and the Relationship to Harm Avoidance. Hum Nat 27, 82–97 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-015-9248-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-015-9248-1

Keywords

Navigation