Human Nature

, Volume 27, Issue 1, pp 68–81 | Cite as

Geographical Cues and Developmental Exposure

Navigational Style, Wayfinding Anxiety, and Childhood Experience in the Faroe Islands
Article

Abstract

The current study assessed potential relationships among childhood wayfinding experience, navigational style, and adult wayfinding anxiety in the Faroe Islands. The Faroe Islands are of interest because they have an unusual geography that may promote the use of an orientational style of navigation (e.g., use of cardinal directions). Faroese adults completed questionnaires assessing (1) their permitted childhood range sizes, (2) the types of navigational strategies they use, and (3) the amount of anxiety they experience when navigating in adulthood. Males had more childhood wayfinding experience, used the orientation strategy at a higher rate, and showed lower levels of wayfinding anxiety. When compared with other cultures, both Faroese women and men appear to embrace orientation strategies at an unusually high rate. Childhood experience was not conclusively linked to later wayfinding anxiety. However, the current findings raise the possibility that children who have particularly small ranges in childhood may be especially anxious when navigating in adulthood.

Keywords

Navigational style Parental permissiveness Restrictive parenting Spatial anxiety Spatial cognition 

References

  1. Baenninger, M., & Newcombe, N. (1989). The role of experience in spatial test performance: a meta-analysis. Sex Roles, 20(5–6), 327–344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Coluccia, E., & Louse, G. (2004). Gender differences in spatial orientation: a review. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24(3), 329–340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Feng, J., Spence, I., & Pratt, J. (2007). Playing an action video game reduces gender differences in spatial cognition. Psychological Science, 18(10), 850–855.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Føroya, H. (2014). Faroe Islands in figures, 2014. Retrieved from http://www.hagstova.fo/sites/default/files/Faroe%20Islands%20in%20figures%202014_0.pdf.
  5. Fyhri, A., Hjorthol, R., Mackett, R. L., Fotel, T. N., & Kyttä, M. (2011). Children's active travel and independent mobility in four countries: development, social contributing trends and measures. Transport Policy, 18(5), 703–710.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Gaffin, D. (1996). In place: spatial and social order in a Faeroe islands community. Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland Press.Google Scholar
  7. Gaini, F. (2013). Lessons of islands: place and identity in the Faroe Islands. Fróðskapur: Faroe University Press.Google Scholar
  8. Hart, R. (1979). Children's experience of place. New York: Irvington.Google Scholar
  9. Herman, J. F., Heins, J. A., & Cohen, D. S. (1987). Children's spatial knowledge of their neighborhood environment. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 8(1), 1–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Hillman, M., Adams, J., & Whitelegg, J. (1990). One false move: A study of children’s independent mobility. London: Policy Studies Institute.Google Scholar
  11. Kyttä, M. (1997). Children’s independent mobility in urban, small town, and rural environments. In R. Cammstra (Ed.), Growing up in a changing urban landscape (pp. 41–52). Assen: Royal Van Gorcum.Google Scholar
  12. Lawton, C. A. (1994). Gender differences in way-finding strategies: relationship to spatial ability and spatial anxiety. Sex Roles, 30(11–12), 765–779.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Lawton, C. A. (1996). Strategies for indoor wayfinding: the role of orientation. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 16(2), 137–145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Lawton, C. A., & Kallai, J. (2002). Gender differences in wayfinding strategies and anxiety about wayfinding: a cross-cultural comparison. Sex Roles, 47(9–10), 389–401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. LeMoyne, T., & Buchanan, T. (2011). Does “hovering” matter? Helicopter parenting and its effect on well-being. Sociological Spectrum, 31(4), 399–418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Massa, L. J., Mayer, R. E., & Bohon, L. M. (2005). Individual differences in gender role beliefs influence spatial ability test performance. Learning and Individual Differences, 15(2), 99–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Matthews, M. H. (1987). Gender, home range and environmental cognition. Transactions of the Institute of British Geographers, 12, 43–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Miller, D. I., & Halpern, D. F. (2014). The new science of cognitive sex differences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(1), 37–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Mitra, R., Faulkner, G. E., Buliung, R. N., & Stone, M. R. (2014). Do parental perceptions of the neighbourhood environment influence children’s independent mobility? Evidence from Toronto, Canada. Urban Studies, 51(16), 3401–3419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Moè, A. (2009). Are males always better than females in mental rotation? Exploring a gender belief explanation. Learning and Individual Differences, 19(1), 21–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Munroe, R. L., & Munroe, R. H. (1971). Effect of environmental experience on spatial ability in an East African society. The Journal of Social Psychology, 83(1), 15–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Nerlove, S. B., Munroe, R. H., & Munroe, R. L. (1971). Effect of environmental experience on spatial ability: a replication. The Journal of Social Psychology, 84(1), 3–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. O'Brien, M., Jones, D., Sloan, D., & Rustin, M. (2000). Children's independent spatial mobility in the urban public realm. Childhood, 7(3), 257–277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Prezza, M. (2007). Children's independent mobility: a review of recent Italian literature. Children, Youth and Environments, 17(4), 293–318.Google Scholar
  25. Prezza, M., Alparone, F. R., Cristallo, C., & Luigi, S. (2005). Parental perception of social risk and of positive potentiality of outdoor autonomy for children: the development of two instruments. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 25(4), 437–453.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Prezza, M., Pilloni, S., Morabito, C., Sersante, C., Alparone, F. R., & Giuliani, M. V. (2001). The influence of psychosocial and environmental factors on children's independent mobility and relationship to peer frequentation. Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology, 11(6), 435–450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Rissotto, A., & Tonucci, F. (2002). Freedom of movement and environmental knowledge in elementary school children. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 22(1), 65–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Rose, K. A., Morgan, I. G., Ip, J., Kifley, A., Huynh, S., Smith, W., & Mitchell, P. (2008). Outdoor activity reduces the prevalence of myopia in children. Ophthalmology, 115(8), 1279–1285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Sandseter, E. B. H., & Kennair, L. E. O. (2011). Children’s risky play from an evolutionary perspective: the anti-phobic effects of thrilling experiences. Evolutionary Psychology, 9(2), 257–284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Schmitz, S. (1997). Gender-related strategies in environmental development: effects of anxiety on wayfinding in and representation of a three-dimensional maze. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 17(3), 215–228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Schug, M. G. (2016). Equal children play best: raising independent Faroese children in the Nordic welfare state. In J. DeLoache, & A. Gottlieb (Eds.), World of babies: imagined childcare guides for nine societies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (in press).Google Scholar
  32. Segrin, C., Woszidlo, A., Givertz, M., & Montgomery, N. (2013). Parent and child traits associated with overparenting. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 32(6), 569–595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Silverman, I., Choi, J., & Peters, M. (2007). The hunter-gatherer theory of sex differences in spatial abilities: data from 40 countries. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36(2), 261–268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. United Nations Statistics Division (2008). Demographic statistics. Retrieved from http://data.un.org/Data.aspx?d=POPandf=tableCode%3A240
  35. Uttal, D. H., Meadow, N. G., Tipton, E., Hand, L. L., Alden, A. R., Warren, C., & Newcombe, N. S. (2013). The malleability of spatial skills: a meta-analysis of training studies. Psychological Bulletin, 139(2), 352–402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Villanueva, K., Giles-Corti, B., Bulsara, M., McCormack, G. R., Timperio, A., et al. (2012). How far do children travel from their homes? Exploring children's activity spaces in their neighborhood. Health and Place, 18(2), 263–273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Webley, P. (1981). Sex differences in home range and cognitive maps in eight-year-old children. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 1(4), 293–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyWidener UniversityChesterUSA

Personalised recommendations