Abstract
In recent years, evolutionary psychologists and anthropologists have debated whether ethnic markers have evolved to solve adaptive problems related to interpersonal coordination or to interpersonal cooperation. In the present study, we add to this debate by exploring how individuals living in a modern society utilize the accents of unfamiliar individuals to make social decisions in hypothetical economic games that measure interpersonal trust, generosity, and coordination. A total of 4603 Danish participants completed a verbal-guise study administered over the Internet. Participants listened to four speakers (two local and two nonlocal) and played a hypothetical Dictator Game, Trust Game, and Coordination Game with each of them. The results showed that participants had greater faith in coordinating successfully with local speakers than with nonlocal speakers. The coordination effect was strong for individuals living in the same city as the particular speakers and weakened as the geographical distance between the participants and the speakers grew. Conversely, the results showed that participants were not more generous toward or more trusting of local speakers compared with nonlocal speakers. Taken together, the results suggest that humans utilize ethnic markers of unfamiliar individuals to coordinate behavior rather than to cooperate.
Similar content being viewed by others
Notes
As a means of effectively discriminating between ingroup and outgroup individuals, accents strike a functional balance between allowing detection of cheaters (a given accent typically cannot be faked in a fleeting encounter) and allowing the accommodation of new individuals into the ethnic group (an accent can typically be acquired through extensive exposure to it). See Cohen 2012.
Experimental studies using behavioral responses to adult speakers’ accents are rare. Using natural experiments with cinema audiences, however, two studies have shown that behavioral compliance to public solicitations (i.e., to fill out questionnaires) made over the cinema’s loudspeakers were affected by the solicitor’s language and accent. In Wales, Bourhis and Giles (1976) found significantly greater compliance with the solicitations when they were made by ingroup speakers as compared to outgroup speakers. Kristansen and Giles (1992) replicated this finding in a Danish sample, but only with adult audiences. Young audiences returned more questionnaires when the solicitations were made by a nonlocal (Standard) speaker.
In essence, this meant that only a fraction of the respondents would listen to locally accented speakers in the most narrow sociolectual linguistic sense. Most participants instead listened to two evidently nonlocal speakers (i.e., speakers living more than 150 km from the participant’s region of origin) and to two speakers with accents that approximated the accent spoken by the participant (i.e., speakers living less than 150 km from the participant’s region of origin).
The most common verbal descriptions made by the speakers include descriptions of children playing on a seesaw, children playing with a ball, children climbing a tree, and the time indicated by a large clock.
Answer labels: (1) No, I don’t think we would; (2) That would probably be completely random; (3) Well, I guess we might; (4) Yes, I’m pretty sure we would; (5) Yes, definitely.
Answer labels: (1) Definitely not; (2) Most likely not; (3) Probably not; (4) Neutral; (5) Probably yes; (6) Most likely yes; (7) Definitely yes.
Two of the sites (www.bt.dk and www.politiken.dk) convey general news (both sites have nearly one million unique users a month each); one site (www.tdc.dk) conveys IT-support and news (700,000 users); one site (www.videnskab.dk) conveys science news (100,000 users); two sites (www.oestrogen.dk and www.femina.dk) specialize in fashion and lifestyles for women (100,000 users each); and one smaller site (www.studerende.au.dk) conveys news for students at the local university (<10,000 users).
More specifically, if participants were born and raised in Jutland, they were assigned two Herning speakers (ingroup) and two Copenhagen speakers (outgroup). Assignments of the particular speakers from each speaker category were random. Similarly, if participants were born and raised in the larger Copenhagen region, they were assigned two Copenhagen speakers and two Herning speakers (outgroup). Finally, if participants were born and raised in Zealand but outside the larger Copenhagen region, they were assigned two Naestved speakers (ingroup) and two Herning speakers (outgroup). Participants born and raised outside the study regions (N = 618) could not be assigned local speakers. They were assigned two Herning speakers and two Copenhagen speakers.
We cannot report standardized regression coefficients (betas) because we also use cluster robust standard errors.
Additional analyses show that in both games, the effect of perceptions of trustworthiness is significantly stronger than the effect of dominance (p < 0.001).
Analyses of the actual choices in the Coordination Game, however, show that the assumption that it is easier to coordinate with ingroup members is false. In the overall sample, the restaurant “Odysseus” constitutes a focal point, with 68% of the sample choosing this restaurant. However, individuals living in the same city as the speaker are slightly more uncoordinated in their choices, with 65% choosing “Odysseus.” This difference is marginally significant (p < 0.10). The disparity between assumptions and actual success underlines that this bias is driven by intuition rather than reason.
References
Ahmed, A. M. (2007). Group identity, social distance and intergroup bias. Journal of Economic Psychology, 28, 324–337.
Aiello, L., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (1993). Neocortex size, group size, and the evolution of language. Current Anthropology, 34, 184–193.
Alesina, A., & La Ferrara, E. (2002). Who trusts others? Journal of Public Economics, 85, 207–234.
Amir, O., Rand, D. G., & Gal, Y. K. (2012). Economic games on the internet: the effect of $1 stakes. PloS One, 7, e31461.
Antal, T., Ohtsuki, H., Wakeley, J., Taylor, P. D., & Nowak, M. A. (2009). Evolution of cooperation by phenotypic similarity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 106, 8597–8600.
Apicella, C. L., Marlowe, F. W., Fowler, J. H., & Christakis, N. A. (2012). Social networks and cooperation in hunter-gatherers. Nature, 481, 497–501.
Axelrod, R., & Hamilton, W. D. (1981). The evolution of cooperation. Science, 211, 1390–1396.
Bacharach, M., & Bernasconi, M. (1997). The variable frame theory of focal points: an experimental study. Games and Economic Behavior, 19, 1–45.
Baker, M. C. (2001). The atoms of language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Barth, F. (1969). Introduction. In F. Barth (Ed.), Ethnic groups and boundaries (pp. 9–38). Boston: Little, Brown.
Bateson, M., Nettle, D., & Roberts, G. (2006). Cues of being watched enhance cooperation in a real-world setting. Biology Letters, 2, 412–414.
Ben-Ner, A., Kramer, A., & Levy, O. (2008). Economic and hypothetical dictator game experiments: incentive effects at the individual level. Journal of Socio-Economics, 37, 1775–1784.
Ben-Ner, A., McCall, B. P., Stephane, M., & Wang, H. (2009). Identity and in-group/out-group differentiation in work and giving behaviors: experimental evidence. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 72, 153–170.
Berg, J., Dickhaut, J., & McCabe, K. (1995). Trust, reciprocity, and social history. Games and Economic Behavior, 10, 122–142.
Bernhard, H., Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2006a). Group affiliation and altruistic norm enforcement. American Economic Review, 96, 217–221.
Bernhard, H., Fischbacher, U., & Fehr, E. (2006b). Parochial altruism in humans. Nature, 442, 912–915.
Boehm, C. (2000). Conflict and the evolution of social control. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 7, 79–101.
Bourhis, R. Y., & Giles, H. (1976). The Language of cooperation in Wales: a field study. Language Sciences, 42, 13–16.
Bowles, S., & Gintis, H. (2004). Persistent parochialism: trust and exclusion in ethnic networks. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 55, 1–23.
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1987). The evolution of ethnic markers. Cultural Anthropology, 2, 65–79.
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2005). The origin and evolution of cultures. New York: Oxford University Press.
Boyd, R., Gintis, H., & Bowles, S. (2010). Coordinated punishment of defectors sustains cooperation and can proliferate when rare. Science, 328, 617–620.
Brown, B. R., & Turner, J. C. (1979). The criss-cross categorization effect in the intergroup discrimination. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 18, 371–383.
Castillo, M., & Petrie, R. (2010). Discrimination in the lab: does information trump appearance? Games and Economic Behavior, 68, 50–59.
Chambers, J. K., Trudgill, P., & Schilling-Estes, N. (2002). The handbook of language variation and change. Malden: Blackwell.
Chapais, B. (2010). The deep structure of human society: primate origins and evolution. In P. M. Kappeler & J. B. Silk (Eds.), Mind the gap: tracing the origins of human universals (pp. 19–51). New York: Springer.
Chen, Y., & Li, S. X. (2009). Group identity and social preferences. American Economic Review, 99, 431–457.
Cohen, E. (2012). The evolution of tag-based cooperation in humans: the case for accent. Current Anthropology, 53, 588–616.
Cohen, E., & Haun, D. (2013). The development of tag-based cooperation via a socially acquired trait. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 230–235.
Colman, A. M. (1997). Salience and focusing in pure coordination games. Journal of Economic Methodology, 4, 61–81.
Colman, A. M. (2003). Cooperation, psychological game theory, and limitations of rationality in social interaction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26, 139–198.
Cooper, R. L., & Fishman, J. A. (1974). The study of language attitudes. International Journal of the Sociology of Language, 1974, 5–20.
Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. New York: Oxford University Press.
De Cremer, D., & Van Vugt, M. (1999). Social identification effects in social dilemmas: a transformation of motives. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 871–893.
DeChamps, J. C., & Doise, W. (1978). Crossed category memberships in intergroup relations. In H. Tajfel (Ed.), Differentiation between social groups (pp. 141–158). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
DeScioli, P., & Kurzban, R. (2013). A solution to the mysteries of morality. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 477–496.
Edwards, J. (2009). Language and identity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Efferson, C., Lalive, R., & Fehr, E. (2008). The coevolution of cultural groups and ingroup favoritism. Science, 321, 1844–1849.
Ejstrup, M. (2011). Perceptionsundersøgelse af moderne danske dialekter. Ord & Sag, 31, 30–41.
Ejstrup, M., & Hansen, G. H. (2004). Vowels in regional variants of Danish (Proceedings, FONETIK 2004). Dept. of Linguistics, Stockholm: University.
Esteban, J., Mayoral, L., & Ray, D. (2012). Ethnicity and conflict: theory and facts. Science, 336, 858–864.
Falk, A., & Zehnder, C. (2013). A city-wide experiment on trust discrimination. Journal of Public Economics, 100, 15–27.
Falk, C. F., Heine, S. J., & Takemura, K. (2014). Cultural variation in the minimal group effect. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 45, 265–281.
Ferraro, P. J., & Cummings, R. G. (2007). Cultural diversity, discrimination, and economic outcomes: an experimental analysis. Econommic Inquiry, 45, 217–232.
Fershtman, C., & Gneezy, U. (2001). Discrimination in a segmented society: an experimental approach. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116, 351–377.
Fish, F. E. (1995). Kinematics of ducklings swimming in formation: consequences of position. Journal of Experimental Zoology, 273, 1–11.
Forsythe, R., Horowitz, J. L., Savin, N. E., & Sefton, M. (1994). Fairness in simple bargaining experiments. Games and Economic Behavior, 6, 347–369.
Gesthuizen, M., van der Meer, T., & Scheepers, P. (2009). Ethnic diversity and social capital in Europe: tests of Putnam’s thesis in European countries. Scandinavian Political Studies, 32, 121–142.
Giles, H., & Bourhis, R. Y. (1976). Methodological Issues in dialect perception: some social psychological perspectives. Anthropological Linguistics, 18, 294–304.
Giles, H., & Powesland, P. F. (1975). Speech style and social evaluation. London: Academic.
Gillis, M. T., & Hettler, P. L. (2007). Hypothetical and real incentives in the ultimatum game and Andreoni’s public goods game: an experimental study. Eastern Economic Journal, 33, 491–510.
Gil-White, F. (2001). Are ethnic groups biological “species” to the human brain? Essentialism in our cognition of some social categories. Current Anthropology, 42, 515–554.
Gluszek, A., & Dovidio, J. F. (2010). The way they speak: a social psychological perspective on the stigma of nonnative accents in communication. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14, 214–237.
Guionnet, S., Nadel, J., Bertasi, E., Sperduti, M., Delaveau, P., & Fossati, P. (2012). Reciprocal imitation: toward a neural basis of social interaction. Cerebral Cortex, 22, 971–978.
Hari, R., & Kujala, M. V. (2009). Brain basis of human social interaction: from concepts to brain imaging. Physiological Review, 89, 453–479.
Hartstone, M., & Augoustinos, M. (1995). The minimal group paradigm: categorization into two versus three groups. European Journal of Social Psychology, 25, 179–193.
Hero, R. (2003). Social capital and racial inequality in America. Perspectives on Politics, 1, 113–122.
Herschenshon, J. (2007). Language development and age. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Hewstone, M., Rubin, M., & Willis, H. (2002). Intergroup bias. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 575–604.
Hill, K. R., Walker, R. S., Božičević, M., Eder, J., Headland, T., Hewlett, B., Hurtado, M. A., Marlowe, F., Wiessneer, P., & Wood, B. (2011). Co-residence patterns in hunter-gatherer societies show unique human social structure. Science, 331, 1286–1289.
Hoffman, E., McCabe, K., & Smith, V. L. (1996). Social distance and other-regarding behavior in dictator games. American Economic Review, 86, 653–660.
Holland, J., Silva, A. S., & Mace, R. (2012). Lost letter measure of variation in altruisitc behaviour in 20 neighbourhoods. PloS One, 7, e43294.
Hooghe, M., Reeskens, T., Stolle, D., & Trappers, A. (2009). Ethnic diversity and generalized trust in Europe: a cross-national multilevel study. Comparative Political Studies, 42, 198–223.
Hughes, S. M., Harrison, M. A., & Gallup, G. G., Jr. (2002). The sound of symmetry: voice as a marker of developmental instability. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 173–180.
Kay, A., & Rissing, S. W. (2005). Division of foraging labor in ants can mediate demands for food and safety. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 58, 165–174.
Kinzler, K. D., Dupoux, E., & Spelke, E. S. (2007). The native language of social cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 104, 12577–12580.
Kinzler, K. D., Shutts, K., DeJesus, J., & Spelke, E. S. (2009). Accent trumps race in guiding children’s social preferences. Social Cognition, 27, 623–634.
Kinzler, K. D., Corriveau, K. H., & Harris, P. L. (2011). Children’s selective trust in native-accented speakers. Developmental Science, 14, 106–111.
Kinzler, K. D., Dupoux, E., & Spelke, E. S. (2012). ‘Native’ objects and collaborators: infants’ object choices and acts of giving reflect favor for native over foreign speakers. Journal of Cognition and Development, 13, 67–81.
Koopmans, R., & Rebers, S. (2009). Collective action in culturally similar and dissimilar groups: an experiment on parochialism, conditional cooperation, and their linkages. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30, 201–211.
Koopmans, R., & Veit, S. (2014). Cooperation in ethnically diverse neighborhoods: a lost-letter experiment. Political Psychology, 35, 379–400.
Kristansen, T., & Giles, H. (1992). Compliance-gaining as a function of accent: public requests in varieties of Danish. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 2, 17–35.
Kristiansen, T. (2009). The macro-level social meanings of late-modern Danish accents. Acta Linguistica Hafniensia, 41, 167–192.
Kuhl, P. K., Stevens, E., Hayashi, A., Deguchi, T., Kiritani, S., & Iverson, P. (2006). Infants show a facilitation effect for native language phonetic perception between 6 and 12 months. Developmental Science, 9, 13–21.
Kuhn, S. L., & Stiner, M. (2007). Paleolithic ornaments: implications for cognition, demography and identity. Diogenes, 54, 40–48.
Kurzban, R., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2001). Can race be erased? Coalitional computation and social categorization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 98, 15387–15392.
Letki, N. (2008). Does diversity erode social cohesion? Social capital and race in British neighborhoods. Political Studies, 56, 99–126.
LeVine, R. A., & Campbell, D. T. (1972). Ethnocentrism: theories of conflict, ethnic attitudes, and group behavior. New York: Wiley.
Leyens, J. P., Yzerbyt, V., & Schadron, G. (1994). Stereotypes and social cognition. London: Sage.
Lopez, J. C., & Lopez, D. (1985). Killer whales (Orcinus orca) of Patagonia, and their behavior of intentional stranding while hunting nearshore. Journal of Mammalogy, 66, 181–183.
Marras, S., Batty, R. S., & Domenici, P. (2011). Information transfer and antipredator maneuvers in schooling herring. Adaptive Behavior, 20, 44–56.
Marwick, B. (2003). Pleistocene exchange networks as evidence for the evolution of language. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 13, 67–81.
Marwick, B. (2005). The interpersonal origins of language: social and linguistic implications of an archaeology approach to language evolution. Language and the Human Sciences, 1, 197–224.
Masuda, N., & Ohtsuki, H. (2007). Tag-based indirect reciprocity by incomplete social information. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 274, 689–695.
McBrearty, S., & Brooks, A. S. (2000). The revolution that wasn’t: a new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. Journal of Human Evolution, 39, 453–563.
McElreath, R., Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2003). Shared norms and the evolution of ethnic markers. Current Anthropology, 44, 122–129.
Mehta, J., Starmer, C., & Sugden, R. (1994). The nature of salience: an experimental investigation of pure coordination games. Amercan Economic Review, 84, 658–673.
Mifune, N., Hashimoto, H., & Yamagishi, T. (2010). Altruism toward in-group members as a reputation mechanism. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31, 109–117.
Moya, C. (2013). Evolved priors for ethnolinguistic categorization: a case study from the Quechua-Aymara boundary in the Peruvian Altiplano. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 265–272.
Nettle, D. (1999). Linguistic diversity. New York: Oxford University Press.
Nettle, D., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (1997). Social markers and the evolution of reciprocal exchange. Current Anthropology, 38, 93–99.
Nettle, D., Pepper, G. V., Jobling, R., & Schroeder, K. B. (2014). Being there: a brief visit to a neighbourhood induces the social attitudes of that neighbourhood. PeerJ, 2, e236.
Oda, R., Niwa, Y., Honma, A., & Hiraishi, K. (2011). An eye-like painting enhances the expectation of a good reputation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32, 166–171.
Oosterhof, N. N., & Todorov, A. (2008). The functional basis of face evaluation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105, 11087–11092.
Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD) (2013). Crisis squeezes income and puts pressure on inequality and poverty: New results from the OECD Income Distribution Database. URL: http://www.oecd.org/els/soc/OECD2013-Inequality-and-Poverty-8p.pdf
Pietraszewski, D., & Schwartz, A. (2014a). Evidence that accent is a dimension of social categorization, not a byproduct of perceptual salience, familiarity, or ease-of-processing. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35, 43–50.
Pietraszewski, D., & Schwartz, A. (2014b). Evidence that accent is a dedicated dimension of social categorization, not a byproduct of coalitional categorization. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35, 51–57.
Pietraszewski, D., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2014). The content of our cooperation, not the color of our skin: an alliance detection system regulates categorization by coalition and race, but not sex. PloS One, 9, e88534.
Putnam, R. (2007). E Pluribus Unum: diversity and community in the twenty-first century: the Johan Skytte Prize lecture. Scandinavian Political Studies, 30, 134–167.
Puts, D. A., Gaulin, S. J. C., & Verdolini, K. (2006). Dominance and the evolution of sexual dimorphism in human voice pitch. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27, 283–296.
Riolo, R. L., Cohen, M. D., & Axelrod, R. (2001). Evolution of cooperation without reciprocity. Nature, 414, 441–443.
Roberts, G., & Sherratt, T. N. (2002). Does similarity breed cooperation? Nature, 418, 499–500.
Schelling, T. (1960). The strategy of conflict. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Scherer, K. R. (1972). Judging personality from voice: a cross-cultural approach to an old issue in interpersonal perception. Journal of Personality, 40, 191–210.
Scherer, K. R. (1986). Vocal affect expression: a review and a model for future research. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 143–165.
Sell, A., Bryant, G. A., Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., Sznycer, D., von Rueden, C., Krauss, A., & Gurven, M. (2010). Adaptations in humans for assessing physical strength from the voice. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 277, 3509–3518.
Shinada, M., Yamagishi, T., & Ohmura, Y. (2004). False friends are worse than bitter enemies: “Altruistic” punishment of in-group members. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25, 379–393.
Shutts, K., Kinzler, K. D., McKee, C. B., & Spelke, E. S. (2009). Social information guides infants’ selection of foods. Journal of Cognition and Development, 10, 1–17.
Simpson, B. (2006). Social identity and cooperation in social dilemmas. Rationality and Society, 18, 443–470.
Smith, E. A. (2010). Communication and coolective action: language and the evolution of human cooperation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31, 231–245.
Stander, P. E. (1992). Cooperative hunting in lions: the role of the individual. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 29, 445–454.
Stolle, D., Soroka, S., & Johnston, R. (2008). When does diversity erode trust? Neighborhood diversity, interpersonal trust and the mediating effect of social interactions. Political Studies, 56, 57–75.
Sugden, R. (1995). A theory of focal points. The Economic Journal, 105, 533–550.
Summerville, A., & Chartier, C. R. (2013). Behavior Research Methods, 45, 116–124.
Tajfel, H., Billig, M., Bundy, R., & Flament, C. (1971). Social categorization in intergroup behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 1, 149–178.
Tanis, M., & Postmes, T. (2005). A social identity approach to trust: interpersonal perception, group membership and trusting behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35, 413–424.
Thornhill, N. (1993). The natural history of inbreeding and outbreeding: Theoretical and empirical perspectives. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675–735.
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2010). Groups in mind: the coalitional roots of war and morality. In H. Høgh-Olesen (Ed.), Human morality and sociality: evolutionary and comparative perspectives (pp. 191–234). New York: Palgrave Macmillan.
Trivers, R. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 35–57.
Trudgill, P. (1974). Sociolinguistics. Harmondsworth: Penguin.
United Nations Development Programme (UNDP) (2013). The rise of the South: Human progress in a diverse world. Human Development Report (summary). URL: http://hdr.undp.org/en/media/HDR2013_EN_Summary.pdf
Van den Berghe, P. L. (1981). The ethnic phenomenon. Westport: Praeger.
Warring, T. M. (2012). Cooperation dynamics in a multiethnic society. A case study from Tamil Nadu. Current Anthropology, 53, 642–649.
Yamagishi, T., & Mifune, N. (2008). Does shared group membership promote altruism? Fear, greed, and reputation. Rationality and Society, 20, 5–30.
Yamagishi, T., Jin, N., & Kiyonari, T. (1999). Bounded generalized reciprocity: ingroup boasting and ingroup favoritism. Advances in Group Processes, 16, 161–197.
Yamagishi, T., Makimura, Y., Foddy, M., Matsuda, M., Kiyonari, T., & Platow, J. M. (2005). Comparisons of Australians and Japanese on group-based cooperation. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 8, 173–190.
Zuckerman, M., & Driver, R. E. (1989). What sounds beautiful is good: the vocal attractiveness stereotype. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 13, 67–82.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Electronic supplementary material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
ESM 1
(DOCX 101 kb)
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Jensen, N.H., Petersen, M.B., Høgh-Olesen, H. et al. Testing Theories about Ethnic Markers. Hum Nat 26, 210–234 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-015-9229-4
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-015-9229-4