Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

More Lessons from the Hadza about Men’s Work

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Unlike other primate males, men invest substantial effort in producing food that is consumed by others. The Hunting Hypothesis proposes this pattern evolved in early Homo when ancestral mothers began relying on their mates’ hunting to provision dependent offspring. Evidence for this idea comes from hunter-gatherer ethnography, but data we collected in the 1980s among East African Hadza do not support it. There, men targeted big game to the near exclusion of other prey even though they were rarely successful and most of the meat went to others, at significant opportunity cost to their own families. Based on Hadza data collected more recently, Wood and Marlowe contest our position, affirming the standard view of men’s foraging as family provisioning. Here we compare the two studies, identify similarities, and show that emphasis on big game results in collective benefits that would not be supplied if men foraged mainly to provision their own households. Male status competition remains a likely explanation for Hadza focus on big game, with implications for hypotheses about the deeper past.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Barclay, P. (2013). Strategies for cooperation in biological markets, especially for humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 164–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumann, O. (1894). Durch Masailand zur Nilquille. Berlin: Reimer (New York: Johnson Reprint, 1968).

  • Bliege Bird, R., & Smith, E. A. (2005). Signaling theory, strategic interaction, and symbolic capital. Current Anthropology, 46(2), 221–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blurton Jones, N. G. (1984). Selfish origin for human food sharing: tolerated theft. Ethology and Sociobiology, 5(1), 1–3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blurton Jones, N. G. (1987). Tolerated theft: suggestions about the ecology and evolution of sharing, hoarding, and scrounging. Social Science Information, 26, 31–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blurton Jones, N. G., Smith, L., Hawkes, K., O'Connell, J. F., & Kamuzora, C. (1992). Demography of the Hadza, an increasing and high density population of savanna foragers. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 89, 1159–1181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blurton Jones, N. G., Hawkes, K., & O'Connell, J. F. (1996). The global process and local ecology: How should we explain differences between the Hadza and the !Kung? In S. Kent (Ed.), Cultural diversity among twentieth century foragers: An African perspective (pp. 159–187). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bunn, H. T. (2001). Hunting, power scavenging and butchering by Hadza foragers and by Plio-Pleistocene Homo. In C. B. Stanford & H. T. Bunn (Eds.), Meat eating and human evolution (pp. 199–219). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bunn, H. T. (2007). Meat made us human. In P. S. Ungar (Ed.), Evolution of the human diet: The known, the unknown and the unknowable (pp. 191–211). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapais, B. (2008). Primeval kinship: How pair-bonding gave birth to human society. Cambridge: Harvaed University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapais, B. (2011). The evolutionary history of pair-bonding and parental collaboration. In C. A. Salmon & T. K. Shackelford (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of evolutionary family psychology (pp. 33–50). New York: Oxford University Press.

  • Coxworth, J. E. (2013). First among equals: male-male competition among the Bardi of northwestern Australia and its implications for human evolution. Ph.D. dissertation. Salt Lake City: University of Utah.

  • Flinn, M. V., Quinlan, R. J., Coe, K., & Ward, C. V. (2007). Evolution of the human family: Cooperative males, long social childhoods, smart mothers, and extended kin networks. In C. A. Salmon & T. K. Shackelford (Eds.), Family relationships: An evolutionary perspective (pp. 16–38). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gurven, M. (2004). To give or to give not: an evolutionary ecology of human food transfers. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(4), 543–583.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gurven, M., & Hill, K. (2009). Why do men hunt? A reevaluation of “Man the Hunter” and the sexual division of labor. Current Anthropology, 50(1), 51–74.

  • Hawkes, K. (1990). Why do men hunt? Some benefits for risky strategies. In E. Cashdan (Ed.), Risk and uncertainty in tribal and peasant economies (pp. 145–166). Boulder: Westview Press.

  • Hawkes, K. (1991). Showing off: tests of an hypothesis about men’s foraging goals. Ethology and Sociobiology, 12, 29–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K. (1992a). Sharing and collective action. In E. A. Smith & B. Winterhalder (Eds.), Evolutionary ecology and human behavior (pp. 269–300). New York: Aldine de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K. (1992b). On sharing and work (comment on article by Bird-David). Current Anthropology, 33(4), 404–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K. (1993a). Why hunter-gatherers work: an ancient version of the problem of public goods. Current Anthropology, 34(4), 341–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K. (1993b). On why male foragers hunt and share food: reply to Hill and Kaplan. Current Anthropology, 34(5), 706–710.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K. (1996). Foraging differences between men and women: Behavioral ecology of the sexual division of labor. In S. Shennan & J. Steele (Eds.), Power, sex and tradition: The archaeology of human ancestry (pp. 283–305). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., & Bliege Bird, R. (2002). Showing off, handicap signaling and the evolution of men’s work. Evolutionary Anthropology, 11, 58–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (1989). Hardworking Hadza grandmothers. In V. Standen & R. A. Foley (Eds.), Comparative socioecology: The behavioural ecology of humans and other mammals (pp. 341–366). London: Basil Blackwell.

  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (1991). Hunting income patterns among the Hadza: big game, common goods, foraging goals, and the evolution of the human diet. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B, 334, 243–251.

  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (1995). Hadza children’s foraging: juvenile dependency, social arrangements and mobility among hunter-gatherers. Current Anthropology, 36(4), 688–700.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (1997). Hadza women’s time allocation, offspring provisioning and the evolution of post-menopausal lifespans. Current Anthropology, 38(4), 551–577.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (2001a). Hadza meat sharing. Evolution & Human Behavior, 22, 113–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (2001b). Hunting and nuclear families: some lessons from the Hadza about men’s work. Current Anthropology, 42(5), 681–709.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., & Coxworth, J. E. (2010). Family provisioning is not the only reason men hunt. Current Anthropology, 52(2), 259–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K. (1982). Hunting and human evolution. Journal of Human Evolution, 11, 521–544.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isaac, G. L. (1978). The food sharing behavior of proto-human hominids. Scientific American, 238, 90–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, H., & Hill, K. (1985). Food sharing among Ache foragers: tests of explanatory hypotheses. Current Anthropology, 26(2), 223–247.

  • Kaplan, H. S., Hill, K., Hawkes, K., & Hurtado, A. M. (1984). Food sharing among Ache hunter-gatherers of eastern Paraguay. Current Anthropology, 25(1), 113–115.

  • Kaplan, H. S., Hill, K. R., Lancaster, J. B., & Hurtado, A. M. (2000). A theory of human life history evolution: diet, intelligence, and longevity. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9, 156–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, H., Gurven, M., Winking, J., Hooper, P. L., & Stieglitz, J. (2010). Learning, menopause, and the human adaptive complex. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1204, 30–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lancaster, J., & Lancaster, C. (1983). Parental investment: the hominid adaptation. In D. Ortner (Ed.), How humans adapt (pp. 33–69). Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution Press.

  • Lovejoy, C. O. (1981). The origin of man. Science, 211, 341–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe, F. (2005). Hunter-gatherers and human evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology, 14, 54–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe, F. (2010). The Hadza: Hunter-gatherers of Tanzania. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith, J. (1982). Evolution and the theory of games. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • O’Connell, J. F., Hawkes, K., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (1988). Hadza hunting, butchering, and bone transport and their archaeological implications. Journal of Anthropological Research, 44, 113–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Connell, J. F., Hawkes, K., Lupo, K., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (2002). Male strategies and Plio-Pleistocene archaeology. Journal of Human Evolution, 43, 831–872.

  • O’Connell, J. F., Hawkes, K., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (1999). Grandmothering and the evolution of Homo erectus. Journal of Human Evolution, 36, 461–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, G. (1998). Competitive altruism: From reciprocity to the handicap principle. Proceedings of the Royal Society, B, 265, 427–431.

  • Rodseth, L. (2012). From bachelor threat to fraternal security: male associations and modular organization in human societies. International Journal of Primatology, 33, 1194–1214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sands, B. (1998). The linguistic relationship between Hadza and Khoisan. In M. Schladt (Ed.), Language, identity, and conceptualization (pp. 265–283). Quellen zur Khoisan-Forschung 15. Koln: Rudiger Koppe.

  • Smith, E. A., & Bliege Bird, R. (2000). Turtle hunting and tombstone opening: public generosity as costly signaling. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 245–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Speth, J. (2010). The paleoanthropology and archaeology of big-game hunting: Protein, fat, or politics? New York: Springer.

  • Tishkoff, S. A., Gonder, M. K., Henn, B. M., et al. (2007). History of click-speaking populations of Africa inferred from mtDNA and Y chromosome genetic variation. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 24, 2180–2195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trivers, R. (1971). On reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 35–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Washburn, S. L., & Lancaster, C. S. (1968). The evolution of hunting. In R. B. Lee & I. DeVore (Eds.), Man the hunter (pp. 293–303). Chicago: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiessner, P. (1996). Leveling the hunter: Constraints on the status quest in foraging societies. In P. Wiessner & S. Wulf (Eds.), Food and the status quest (pp. 171–191). Oxford: Berghahn Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winterhalder, B. (1986). Diet choice, risk, and sharing in a stochastic environment. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 5, 369–392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, B. M., & Marlowe, F. W. (2013). Household and kin provisioning by Hadza men. Human Nature, 24, 280–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodburn, J. (1968a). An introduction to Hadza ecology. In R. B. Lee & I. DeVore (Eds.), Man the hunter (pp. 49–55). Chicago: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodburn, J. (1968b). Stability and flexibility in Hadza residential groupings. In R. B. Lee & I. DeVore (Eds.), Man the hunter (pp. 103–110). Chicago: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodburn, J. (1979). Minimal politics: the political organization of the Hadza of Tanzania. In W. A. Shack & P. S. Cohen (Eds.), Politics in leadership (pp. 244–266). Oxford: Clarendon.

  • Woodburn, J. (1982). Egalitarian societies. Man, 17, 431–451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodburn, J. (1998). Sharing is not a form of exchange: An analysis of property-sharing in immediate-return hunter-gatherer societies. In C. M. Hann (Ed.), Property relations: Renewing the anthropological tradition (pp. 48–63). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zilhmann, A. (2013). Engendering human evolution. In D. Bolger (Ed.), A companion to gender prehistory (pp. 23–44). Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Steve Beckerman, Ted Coxworth, and Sarah Hrdy for clarifying suggestions.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kristen Hawkes.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J.F. & Blurton Jones, N.G. More Lessons from the Hadza about Men’s Work. Hum Nat 25, 596–619 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-014-9212-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-014-9212-5

Keywords

Navigation