Advertisement

Human Nature

, Volume 25, Issue 1, pp 100–120 | Cite as

Using Multilevel Models to Estimate Variation in Foraging Returns

Effects of Failure Rate, Harvest Size, Age, and Individual Heterogeneity
  • Richard McElreath
  • Jeremy Koster
Article

Abstract

Distributions of human foraging success across age have implications for many aspects of human evolution. Estimating the distribution of foraging returns is complicated by (1) the zero-inflated nature of hunting returns, as many if not most trips fail, and (2) the substantial variation among hunters, independent of age. We develop a multilevel mixture analysis of human foraging data to address these difficulties. Using a previously published 20-year record of hunts by 147 individual Aché hunters in eastern Paraguay, we estimate returns-by-age functions for both hunting failures and the size of harvests, while also estimating the heterogeneity among hunters. Consistent with previous analyses, we find that most hunters peak around 40 years of age. We can also show, however, that much more of the variation among Aché hunters arises from heterogeneity in failure rates (zero returns), not harvest sizes. We also introduce a new R package, glmer2stan, to assist in defining and fitting similar multilevel mixture models.

Keywords

Human behavioral ecology Foraging Multilevel modeling Life history 

Notes

Acknowledgements

Thanks to Mark Grote, Jamie Holland Jones, Bruce Winterhalder, members of the UC Davis Cultural Evolution and Human Behavioral Ecology labs, and two anonymous reviewers for advice and comments. Bob Carpenter and the members of the Stan Development Team helped us improve the efficiency of our Stan code. Keith O’Rourke suggested the analogy between Whorf’s gasoline drums and uninformative priors. Nicholas Blurton-Jones, back in 1997, encouraged RM to find a solution to modeling imbalanced field data, leading him eventually to Bayesian data analysis.

References

  1. Alvard, M. (1995). Shotguns and sustainable hunting in the neotropics. Oryx, 29, 58–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bliege Bird, R., & Bird, D. W. (2002). Constraints of knowing or constraints of growing? Fishing and collecting by the children of Mer. Human Nature, 13(2), 239–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Boyd, R. (1992). The evolution of reciprocity when conditions vary. In A. Harcourt & F. DeWaal (Eds.), Coalitions and alliances in humans and other animals (pp. 473–489). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  4. Browne, W. J., Goldstein, H., & Rasbash, J. (2001). Multiple membership multiple classification (MMMC) models. Statistical Modelling, 1, 103–124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Burnham, K., & Anderson, D. (2002). Model selection and multimodel inference: A practical information-theoretic approach (2nd ed.). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  6. Cashdan, E. (Ed.). (1990). Risk and uncertainty in tribal and peasant economies. Boulder: Westview Press.Google Scholar
  7. Claeskens, G., & Hjort, N. (2008). Model selection and model averaging. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Efron, B., & Morris, C. (1975). Data analysis using Stein’s estimator and its generalizations. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 70, 311–319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Fang, Y. (2011). Asymptotic equivalence between cross-validations and Akaike information criteria in mixed-effects models. Journal of Data Science, 9, 15–21.Google Scholar
  10. Gelman, A., & Hill, J. (2007). Data analysis using regression and multilevel/hierarchical models. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  11. Gelman, A., Carlin, J. C., Stern, H. S., & Rubin, D. B. (2004). Bayesian data analysis (2nd ed.). New York: Chapman & Hall/CRC.Google Scholar
  12. Gelman, A., Jakulin, A., Pittau, M. G., & Su, T.-S. (2008). A weakly informative default prior distribution for logistic and other regression models. Annals of Applied Statistics, 2(4), 1360–1383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Gigerenzer, G. (1991). From tools to theories: a heuristic of discovery in cognitive psychology. Psychological Review, 98(2), 254–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Green, E. J., & Porter, R. H. (1984). Noncooperative collusion under imperfect price information. Econometrica, 52(1), 87–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Gurven, M. (2006). The evolution of contingent cooperation. Current Anthropology, 47, 185–192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Gurven, M., Hill, K., Kaplan, H., Hurtado, A., & Lyles, R. (2000). Food transfers among Hiwi foragers of Venezuela: tests of reciprocity. Human Ecology, 28(2), 171–217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Gurven, M., Kaplan, H., & Gutierrez, M. (2006). How long does it take to become a proficient hunter? Implications for the evolution of delayed growth. Journal of Human Evolution, 51, 454–470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hawkes, K., & Bliege Bird, R. (2002). Showing off, handicap signaling, and the evolution of men’s work. Evolutionary Anthropology, 11, 58–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hill, K. (2002). Altruistic cooperation during foraging by the Ache, and the evolved human predisposition to cooperate. Human Nature, 13(1), 105–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Hill, K., & Kintigh, K. (2009). Can anthropologists distinguish good and poor hunters? Implications for hunting hypotheses, sharing conventions, and cultural transmission. Current Anthropology, 50(3), 369–377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Hill, K., McMillan, G., & Fariña, R. F. (2003). Hunting-related changes in game encounter rates from 1994 to 2001 in the Mbaracayu reserve, Paraguay. Conservation Biology, 17(5), 1312–1323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. House, B. R., Silk, J. B., Henrich, J., Barrett, H. C., Scelza, B., Boyette, A., et al. (2013). The ontogeny of prosocial behavior across diverse societies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA). doi: 10.1073/pnas.1221217110.Google Scholar
  23. Jones, J., Bird, R., & Bird, D. (2013). To kill a kangaroo: understanding the decision to pursue high-risk/high-gain resources. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.1210.Google Scholar
  24. Kaplan, H. S., Hill, K. R., Lancaster, J. B., & Hurtado, A. M. (2000). A theory of human life history evolution: diet, intelligence, and longevity. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9, 156–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Kaplan, H. S., Hooper, P. L., & Gurven, M. (2009). The evolutionary and ecological roots of human social organization. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 3289–3299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Kaplan, H. S., Schniter, E., Smith, V. L., & Wilson, B. J. (2012). Risk and the evolution of human exchange. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 279(1740), 2930–2935.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Koster, J. (2008). Hunting with dogs in Nicaragua: an optimal foraging approach. Current Anthropology, 49, 935–944.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Koster, J. M., & Tankersley, K. B. (2012). Heterogeneity of hunting ability and nutritional status among domestic dogs in lowland Nicaragua. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 109, E463–E470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Le, S., & Boyd, R. (2007). Evolutionary dynamics of the continuous iterated prisoner’s dilemma. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 245, 258–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Lewandowski, D., Kurowicka, D., & Joe, H. (2009). Generating random correlation matrices based on vines and extended onion method. Journal of Multivariate Analysis, 100, 1989–2001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Lunn, D., Jackson, C., Best, N., Thomas, A., & Spiegelhalter, D. (2013). The BUGS Book: A practical introduction to Bayesian analysis. Boca Raton: CRC Press.Google Scholar
  32. McCullagh, P., & Nelder, J. A. (1989). Generalized linear models (2nd ed.). New York: Chapman and Hall/CRC Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. McElreath, R. (2012). glmer2stan: Rstan models defined by glmer formulas, version 0.96. Available online at http://xcelab.net/rm/.
  34. Neal, R. (2011). MCMC using Hamiltonian dynamics. In G. Brooks & M. Jones (Eds.), Handbook of Markov Chain Monte Carlo (pp. 113–162). Boca Raton: Chapman and Hall/CRC Press.Google Scholar
  35. Plummer, M. (2003). JAGS: a program for analysis of Bayesian graphical models using Gibbs sampling. In Proceedings of the 3rd International Workshop on Distributed Statistical Computing (DSC 2003). Available online at http://www.r-project.org/conferences/DSC-2003/Proceedings/.
  36. Plummer, M. (2008). Penalized loss functions for Bayesian model comparison. Biostatistics, 9(3), 523–539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Rubin, D. B. (1976). Inference and missing data (with discussion). Biometrika, 63, 581–592.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Spiegelhalter, D. J., Best, N. G., Carlin, B. P., & va der Linde, A. (2002). Bayesian measures of model complexity and fit. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society B, 64, 583–639.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Stan Development Team. (2012). Stan: A C++ library for probability and sampling, version 1.0.2. Available online at http://mc-stan.org/.
  40. Stephens, D. W., & Charnov, E. L. (1982). Optimal foraging: some simple stochastic models. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 10, 251–263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Stephens, D. W., & Krebs, J. R. (1986). Foraging theory. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  42. Vaida, F., & Blanchard, S. (2005). Conditional Akaike information for mixed-effects models. Biometrika, 92(2), 351–370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Walker, R., Hill, K., Kaplan, H., & McMillan, G. (2002). Age-dependency in hunting ability among the Ache of eastern Paraguay. Journal of Human Evolution, 42, 639–657.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Whorf, B. (1941). The relation of habitual thought and behavior to language. In L. Spier (Ed.), Language, culture, and personality, essays in memory of Edward Sapir (pp. 75–93). Menasha: Sapir Memorial Publication Fund.Google Scholar
  45. Winterhalder, B. (1990). Open field, common pot: harvest variability and risk avoidance in agricultural and foraging societies. In E. A. Cashdan (Ed.), Risk and uncertainty in tribal and peasant economies (pp. 67–87). Boulder: Westview Press.Google Scholar
  46. Winterhalder, B., Kennett, D. J., Grote, M. N., & Bartruff, J. (2010). Ideal free settlement of California’s northern Channel Islands. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 29, 469–490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Anthropology and Center for Population BiologyUniversity of California, DavisDavisUSA
  2. 2.Department of AnthropologyUniversity of CincinnatiCincinnatiUSA

Personalised recommendations