Human Nature

, Volume 24, Issue 4, pp 351–374 | Cite as

Teaching and the Life History of Cultural Transmission in Fijian Villages

Article

Abstract

Much existing literature in anthropology suggests that teaching is rare in non-Western societies, and that cultural transmission is mostly vertical (parent-to-offspring). However, applications of evolutionary theory to humans predict both teaching and non-vertical transmission of culturally learned skills, behaviors, and knowledge should be common cross-culturally. Here, we review this body of theory to derive predictions about when teaching and non-vertical transmission should be adaptive, and thus more likely to be observed empirically. Using three interviews conducted with rural Fijian populations, we find that parents are more likely to teach than are other kin types, high-skill and highly valued domains are more likely to be taught, and oblique transmission is associated with high-skill domains, which are learned later in life. Finally, we conclude that the apparent conflict between theory and empirical evidence is due to a mismatch of theoretical hypotheses and empirical claims across disciplines, and we reconcile theory with the existing literature in light of our results.

Key words

Cultural transmission Human evolution Teaching Learning Childhood 

Supplementary material

12110_2013_9180_MOESM1_ESM.docx (244 kb)
ESM 1(DOCX 244 kb)

References

  1. Aoki, K., Lehmann, L., & Feldman, M. W. (2011). Rates of cultural change and patterns of cultural accumulation in stochastic models of social transmission. Theoretical Population Biology, 79, 192–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Arno, A. (1990). Disentangling indirectly: The joking debate in Fijian social control. In K. A. Watson-Gegeo & G. M. White (Eds.), Disentangling conflict discourse in Pacific societies (pp. 241–269). Palo Alto: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  3. Aunger, R. (2000). The life history of culture learning in a face-to-face society. Ethos, 28(3), 445–481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bird, D. W., & Bliege-Bird, R. (2002). Children on the reef: Slow learning or strategic foraging? Human Nature, 13(2), 269–297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bliege-Bird, R., & Bird, D. W. (2002). Constraints of knowing or constraints of growing? Fishing and collecting by the children of Mer. Human Nature, 13(2), 239–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bock, J. (2002). Learning, life history, and productivity: Children’s lives in the Okavango Delta, Botswana. Human Nature, 12, 161–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Borofsky, R. (1987). Making history: Pukapukan and anthropological constructions of knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  8. Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  9. Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1988). An evolutionary model of social learning: The effects of spatial and temporal variation. In T. Zentall & B. G. Galef (Eds.), Social learning: A psychological and biological approaches (pp. 29–48). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc.Google Scholar
  10. Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1996). Why culture is common but cultural evolution is rare. Proceedings of the British Academy, 88, 73–96. http://www.britac.ac.uk/pubs/cat/pba88.cfm.Google Scholar
  11. Boyd, R., Richerson, P. J., & Henrich, J. (2011). The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 108, 10918–10925.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Brison, K. T. (1999). Hierarchy in the world of Fijian children. Ethnology, 38, 97–119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Brison, K. T. (2007). Imagining modernity in rural Fiji. Ethnology, 42, 335–348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Caro, T., & Hauser, M. (1992). Is there teaching in nonhuman animals? Quarterly Review of Biology, 67(2), 151–174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., & Feldman, M. W. (1981). Cultural transmission and evolution: A quantitative approach. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  16. Coy, M. (1989). Apprenticeship: From theory to methods and back again. Albany: SUNY Albany Press.Google Scholar
  17. Dakuidreketi, M.R. (2006). Contexts of science teaching and learning in Fiji primary schools: A comparative study of ethnic Fijian and Indo-Fijian communities. (Doctoral Dissertation). University of Canterbury School of Educational Studies and Human Development.Google Scholar
  18. Enquist, M., Eriksson, K., & Ghirlanda, S. (2007). Critical social learning: A solution to Rogers’s paradox of nonadaptive culture. American Anthropologist, 109, 727–734.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Feldman, M. W., Aoki, K., Kumm, J. (1996). Individual versus social learning: Evolutionary analysis in a fluctuating environment. Santa Fe Institute Working Paper. Available online at http://www.santafe.edu/research/working-papers/abstract/158eef061eba1f42429d322426508c20/
  20. Gatty, R. (2009). Fijian-English dictionary, with notes on Fijian culture and natural history. Suva: Oceania Printers.Google Scholar
  21. Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. (2006). Sylvia’s recipe: The role of imitation and pedagogy in the transmission of cultural knowledge. In N. J. Enfield & S. C. Levenson (Eds.), Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition, and human interaction (pp. 229–255). Berg: Oxford.Google Scholar
  22. Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. (2011). Natural pedagogy and an evolutionary adaptation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366, 1149–1157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Henrich, J. (2009). The evolution of costly displays, cooperation, and religion: Credibility enhancing displays and their implications for cultural evolution. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30, 244–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Henrich, J., & Boyd, R. (1998). The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence of between-group differences. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19, 215–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Henrich, J., & Broesch, J. (2011). On the nature of cultural transmission networks: Evidence from Fijian villages for adaptive learning biases. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366, 1139–1148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 165–196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Henrich, J., & Henrich, N. (2010). The evolution of cultural adaptations: Fijian food taboos protect against dangerous marine toxins. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 277, 3715–3724.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Henrich, J., Ensminger, J., McElreath, R., Barr, A., Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., et al. (2010a). Markets, religion, community size, and the evolution of fairness and punishment. Science, 327, 1480–1484. doi:10.1126/science.1182238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010b). The weirdest people in the world. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 61–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hewlett, B. S., & Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (1986). Cultural transmission among Aka pygmies. American Anthropologist, 88, 922–934.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Hewlett, B. S., & Lamb, M. E. (2007). Hunter-gatherer childhoods: Evolutionary, development, and cultural perspectives. New Brunswick: Aldine/Transaction.Google Scholar
  32. Hewlett, B. S., Fouts, H. N., Boyette, A. H., & Hewlett, B. L. (2011). Social learning among Congo basin hunter-gatherers. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366, 1168–1178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Hill, K., Barton, M., & Hurtado, A. M. (2009). The emergence of human uniqueness: Characters underlying behavioral modernity. Evolutionary Anthropology, 18, 187–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Hocart, A. M. (1929). Lau Islands, Fiji. Honolulu: Bernice P. Bishop Museum.Google Scholar
  35. Hoppitt, W., & Laland, K. (2008). Social processes influencing learning in animals: A review of the evidence. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 38, 105–165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Hoppitt, W. J. E., Brown, G. R., Kendal, R., Rendell, L., Thornton, A., Webster, M. M., et al. (2008). Lessons from animal teaching. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 23, 486–493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Jaswal, V. K., Croft, A., Carrington, S., Alison, R., & Cole, C. A. (2010). Young children have a specific, highly robust bias to trust testimony. Psychological Science, 21, 1541–1547.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Kameda, T., & Nakanishi, D. (2003). Does social/cultural learning increase human adaptability? Rogers’s question revisited. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 242–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Kaplan, H., Hill, K., Lancaster, J., & Hurtado, A. M. (2000). A theory of human life history evolution: Diet, intelligence, and longevity. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9, 156–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Koenig, M., & Harris, P. (2007). The basis of epistemic trust: Reliable testimony or reliable sources? Episteme, 4, 264–284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Lancy, D. (2008). Anthropology of childhood: Cherubs, chattel, changelings. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  42. Lancy, D., & Grove, M. A. (2010). The role of adults in children’s learning. In D. F. Lancy, J. Bock, & S. Gaskins (Eds.), The anthropology of learning in childhood (pp. 145–179). Lanham: AltaMira Press.Google Scholar
  43. Lancy, D. F., & Grove, M. A. (2011). Getting noticed: Middle childhood in cross-cultural perspective. Human Nature, 22, 281–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. McElreath, R., & Strimling, P. (2008). When natural selection favors imitation of parents. Current Anthropology, 49, 307–316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Nabobo-Baba, U. (2006). Knowing & learning: An indigenous Fijian approach. Suva: Institute of Pacific Studies Press.Google Scholar
  46. Nakahashi, W., Wakano, J., & Henrich, J. (2012). Conditions for conformist transmission: Spatial variation and the number of cultural traits favors conformist biases over individual learning, unbiased transmission, and payoff-biased transmission. Human Nature, 23, 386–418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Nayacakalou, R. (1975). Leadership in Fiji. Melbourne: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  48. Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B. B. (1984). Language acquisition and socialization. In R. A. Shweder & R. A. Levine (Eds.), Culture theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion (pp. 263–301). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  49. Perreault, C., Cristina, M., & Robert, B. (2012). A Bayesian approach to the evolution of social learning. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33, 449–459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Ravuvu, A. (1983). The Fijian way. Suva: University of South Pacific Press.Google Scholar
  51. Rendell, L., Fogarty, L., & Laland, K. (2010). Rogers’s paradox recast and resolved: Population structure and the evolution of social learning strategies. Evolution, 64, 534–548.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Reyes-García, V., Broesch, J., Calvet-Mir, L., Fuentes-Peláez, N., McDade, T. W., Parsa, S., et al. (2009). Cultural transmission of ethnobotanical knowledge and skills: An empirical analysis from an Amerindian society. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30, 274–285. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.02.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Ritchie, J. B., & Ritchie, J. E. (1979). Growing up in Polynesia. Worthing: Littlehampton Book Svc.Google Scholar
  54. Rogers, A. R. (1988). Does biology constrain culture? American Anthropologist, 90, 819–831.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Sahlins, M. D. (1962). Moala: Culture and nature on a Fijian island. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
  56. Shennan, S., & Steele, J. (1999). Cultural learning in hominids: A behavioural ecological approach. In H. Box & K. Gibson (Eds.), Mammalian social learning: Comparative and ecological perspectives (pp. 367–388). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  57. Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1995). Relevance: Communication and cognition (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  58. Sperber, D., Clement, F., Heintz, C., Mascaro, O., Mercier, H., Origgi, G., et al. (2010). Epistemic vigilance. Mind & Language, 25, 359–393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Thornton, A., & Raihani, N. (2008). The evolution of teaching. Animal Behaviour, 75, 1823–1836.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Toren, C. (1990). Making sense of hierarchy. Atlantic Highlands: Athlone Press.Google Scholar
  61. Veramu, J. (1992). Let’s do it our way: A case study of participatory education in a rural Fijian school and community. Suva: Institute of Pacific Studies Press.Google Scholar
  62. Wakano, J. Y., & Aoki, K. (2007). Do social learning and conformist bias coevolve? Henrich and Boyd revisited. Theoretical Population Biology, 72, 504–512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. White, C. M. (2007). Schooling in Fiji. In C. Campbell & G. Sherington (Eds.), Going to school in Oceania (pp. 79–132). Westport: Greenwood Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  • Michelle A. Kline
    • 1
  • Robert Boyd
    • 2
    • 3
  • Joseph Henrich
    • 4
  1. 1.Department of AnthropologyUniversity of California Los AngelesLos AngelesUSA
  2. 2.School of Human Evolution and Social ChangeArizona State UniversityPhoenixUSA
  3. 3.Sante Fe InstituteSanta FeUSA
  4. 4.Departments of Psychology and EconomicsUniversity of British ColumbiaVancouverCanada

Personalised recommendations