Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The Social Strategy Game

Resource Competition within Female Social Networks among Small-scale Forager-Horticulturalists

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper examines social determinants of resource competition among Tsimane Amerindian women of Bolivia. We introduce a semi-anonymous experiment (the Social Strategy Game) designed to simulate resource competition among women. Information concerning dyadic social relationships and demographic data were collected to identify variables influencing resource competition intensity, as measured by the number of beads one woman took from another. Relationship variables are used to test how the affiliative or competitive aspects of dyads affect the extent of prosociality in the game. Using a mixed-modeling procedure, we find that women compete with those with whom they are quarreling over accusations of meat theft, mate competition, and rumor spreading. They also compete with members of their social network and with those who were designated as cooperative helpers or as close kin. Women take fewer beads from desired friends, neighbors, and from those viewed as enemies. We interpret favoritism toward enemies as resulting from fear of retribution. Our results suggest that social relations among women are multifaceted and often cannot be simplified by exclusive focus on genetic relatedness, physical proximity, or reciprocity. We argue that a complex understanding of cooperation and competition among women may require important contextual information concerning relationship history in addition to typical features of resource ecology.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anderson, C. M. (2000). The persistence of polygyny as an adaptive response to poverty and oppression in apartheid South Africa. Cross-Cultural Research, 34, 99–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, K. G., Kaplan, H., & Lancaster, J. (1999). Parental care by genetic and step fathers, I: Reports by men in Albuquerque, New Mexico. Evolution and Human Behavior, 20, 433–451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Archer, J., & Coyne, S. M. (2005). An integrated review of indirect, relational, and social aggression. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 212–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bateman, A. J. (1948). Intra-sexual selection in Drosophila. Heredity, 2, 349–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorkqvist, K., Lagerspetz, K. M. J., & Kaukiainen, A. (1992). Do girls manipulate and boys fight developmental trends in regard to direct and indirect aggression? Aggressive Behavior, 18, 117–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borgerhoff Mulder, M. (1987). Resources and reproductive success in women, with an example from the Kipsigis. Journal of Zoology, 213, 489–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. M. (1995). Psychological sex differences: Origins through sexual selection. American Psychologist, 50, 164–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, A. (1995). A few good men: Evolutionary psychology and female adolescent aggression. Ethology and Sociobiology, 16, 99–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, A. (2002). A mind of her own: The evolutionary psychology of women. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, A., Muncer, S., Guy, A., & Banhim, M. (1996). Social representation of aggression: Crossing the sex barrier. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 135–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cashdan, E. (1998). Are men more competitive than women? British Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 213–229.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cashdan, E. (2003). Hormones and competitive aggression in women. Aggressive Behavior, 29, 107–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chagnon, N. A. (1990). Reproductive and somatic conflicts of interest in the genesis of violence and warfare among tribesmen. In J. Haas (Ed.), Anthropology of war (pp. 77–104). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cordain, L., Brand Miller, J., Boyd Eaton, S., et al. (2000). Plant-animal subsistence ratios and macronutrient energy estimations in worldwide hunter-gatherer diets. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 71, 682–692.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crick, N. R. (1995). Relational aggression: The role of intent attributions, feelings of distress, and provocation type. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 313–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cronk, L. (1994). Wealth, status and reproductive success among the Mukogodo of Kenya. American Anthropologist, 93, 345–360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. London: J. Murray.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eckel, C., & Grossman, P. (1998). Are women less selfish than men? Evidence from dictator experiments. Economic Journal, 108, 726–735.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flinn, M., Geary, D., & Ward, C. (2005). Ecological dominance, social competition and coalitionary arms races: Why humans evolved extraordinary intelligence. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 10–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forsythe, R., Horowitz, J. L., Savin, N. E., & Sefton, M. (1994). Fairness in simple bargaining experiments. Games and Economic Behavior, 6, 347–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gurven, M. (2004). Economic games among the Amazonian Tsimane: Exploring the roles of market access, costs of giving, and cooperation on pro-social game behavior. Experimental Economics, 7, 5–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gurven, M., & Walker, R. (2006). Energetic demand of multiple dependents and the evolution of slow human growth. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 273, 835–841.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gurven, M., & Winking, J. (2008). Collective action in action: Prosocial behavior in and out of the laboratory. American Anthropologist, 110, 179–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gurven, M., Kaplan, H., & Gutierrez, M. (2006). How long does it take to become a proficient hunter? Implications for the evolution of extended development and long lifespan. Journal of Human Evolution, 51, 454–470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gurven, M., Kaplan, H., & Supa, A. Z. (2007). Mortality experience of Tsimane Amerindians of Bolivia: Regional variation and temporal trends. American Journal of Human Biology, 19, 376–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gurven, M., Zanolini, A., & Schniter, E. (2008). Culture sometimes matters: Intra-cultural variation in division norms among Tsimane Amerindians, real or spurious? Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 67, 587–607.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., Blurton Jones, N. G., Alvarez, H., & Charnov, E. L. (1998). Grandmothering, menopause, and the evolution of human life histories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95, 1336–1339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., et al. (2001). In search of Homo economicus: Experiments in 15 small-scale societies. American Economic Review, 91, 73–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., et al. (Eds.) (2004). Foundations of human sociality: Economic experiments and ethnographic evidence from fifteen small-scale societies. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Hess, N. C. (2006). Informational warfare: The evolution of female coalitions and gossip. Ph.D. dissertation, University of California, Santa Barbara.

  • Hess, N. C., & Hagen, E. H. (2006a). Psychological adaptations for assessing gossip believability. Human Nature, 17, 337–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hess, N. C., & Hagen, E. H. (2006b). Sex differences in indirect aggression: Psychological evidence from young adults. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27, 231–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hurtado, A. M., Hawkes, K., Hill, K., & Kaplan, H. (1985). Female subsistence strategies among Ache hunter-gatherers of eastern Paraguay. Human Ecology, 13, 1–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Janson, C. H. (2000). Primate socio-ecology: The end of a golden age. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9, 73–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, R. L. (1995). The foraging spectrum: Diversity in hunter-gatherer lifeways. Washington: Smithsonian Institution Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamphere, L. (1977). The Navajo cultural system: An analysis of concepts of cooperation and autonomy and their relation to gossip and witchcraft. In M. Opler (Ed.), Apachean culture history and ethnology (pp. 91–114). Tucson: University of Arizona Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamphere, L., & Rosaldo, M. Z. (Eds.) (1974). Woman, culture and society. Palo Alto: Stanford University Press.

  • Lancaster, J. B. (1991). A feminist and evolutionary biologist looks at women. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, 34, 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levitt, S., & List, J. (2009). What do laboratory experiments tell us about the real world? Journal of Economic Perspectives, 21, 153–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe, F. (2001). Male contribution to diet and female reproductive success among foragers. Current Anthropology, 42, 755–760.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGraw, K. J. (2002). Environmental predictors of geographic variation in human mating preferences. Ethology, 108, 303–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parker, G. A., Baker, R. R., & Smith, F. C. F. (1972). The origin and evolution of gamete dimorphism and the male-female phenomenon. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 36, 529–553.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pelegrini, A. D., & Bartini, M. (2001). Dominance in early adolescent boys: Affiliative and aggressive dimensions and possible functions. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 47, 142–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, G., & Sheratt, T. N. (1998). Development of cooperative relationships through increasing investment. Nature, 394, 175–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rucas, S. L. (2004). Female intrasexual social behaviors among the Tsimane of Bolivia. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Anthropology, University of New Mexico.

  • Silk, J. (1983). Local resource competition and facultative adjustment of sex ratios in relation to competitive abilities. American Naturalist, 121, 56–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silk, J. B., Alberts, S. C., & Altmann, J. (2003). Social bonds of female baboons enhance infant survival. Science, 302, 1231–1234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silk, J. B., Alberts, S. C., & Altmann, J. (2004). Patterns of coalition formation by adult female baboons in Amboseli, Kenya. Animal Behaviour, 67, 573–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thaler, R. (1980). Toward a positive theory of consumer choice. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 1, 39–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Underwood, M. K. (2003). Social aggression among girls. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, J. D., Flinn, M. V., & England, B. G. (2002). Hormonal response to competition among male coalitions. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 437–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waynforth, D. (1999). Differences in time use for mating and nepotistic effort as a function of male attractiveness in rural Belize. Evolution and Human Behavior, 20, 19–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (1985). Competitiveness, risk-taking, and violence: The young male syndrome. Ethology and Sociobiology, 6, 59–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham, R. W., & Peterson, D. (1996). Demonic males: Apes and the origins of human violence. New York: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yamaguchi, H. (1992). Formations of grand coalition when consciousness of intragroup competition is activated. Japanese Journal of Psychology, 62, 357–363.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the Tsimane women of our host communities for their gracious participation and wonderful camaraderie. This research was funded by LAII Field Research Grants, provided by the Tinker Foundation, and by NSF grant BCS-013274. We would also like to thank Dan Fessler, Steve Gangestad, and our anonymous reviewers for their helpful criticisms.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stacey L. Rucas.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Rucas, S.L., Gurven, M., Kaplan, H. et al. The Social Strategy Game. Hum Nat 21, 1–18 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-010-9079-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-010-9079-z

Keywords

Navigation