Skip to main content
Log in

Sex Differences in Detecting Sexual Infidelity

Results of a Maximum Likelihood Method for Analyzing the Sensitivity of Sex Differences to Underreporting

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Despite the importance of extrapair copulation (EPC) in human evolution, almost nothing is known about the design features of EPC detection mechanisms. We tested for sex differences in EPC inference-making mechanisms in a sample of 203 young couples. Men made more accurate inferences (φmen = 0.66, φwomen = 0.46), and the ratio of positive errors to negative errors was higher for men than for women (1.22 vs. 0.18). Since some may have been reluctant to admit EPC behavior, we modeled how underreporting could have influenced these results. These analyses indicated that it would take highly sex-differentiated levels of underreporting by subjects with trusting partners for there to be no real sex difference. Further analyses indicated that men may be less willing to harbor unresolved suspicions about their partners’ EPC behavior, which may explain the sex difference in accuracy. Finally, we estimated that women underreported their own EPC behavior (10%) more than men (0%).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In principle, we could also treat the inference (rather than the certainty associated with it) as a continuous variable. However, this leads to unnatural coding of the correctness of inferences. Suppose, for instance, that person A thought there was an 80% chance that her partner had had an affair, whereas person B reported a 90% chance that her partner had had an affair. Assume also that the partners of both of these people admitted to having EPCs. Under this coding scheme, we would say that A is 80% correct, B is 90% correct, and B is 10% more correct than A. However, we don’t believe that people think this way. A and B would probably say that they are both inferring that their partners had affairs, and that both were correct. But they would probably agree that B was more certain about her inference than A.

  2. It is possible that some subjects who answered “yes” to the “to your knowledge” question were not actually 100% certain that their partners had affairs. However, it seems reasonable to assume that if there were such subjects, they would still have been at least 50% certain that their partner had an affair. Thus, this would not influence our coding of them as being suspicious that their partner had an affair or the results of our analyses about accuracy and error bias that we report. It would only influence the certainty of that inference, and the results that depend on certainty.

  3. Of course, subjects might have had a different view of whether their own extrapair sexual behavior was illicit, but they were only asked to provide information on the affair behavior of their partners, not themselves.

References

  • Anderson, K. G., Kaplan, H., & Lancaster, J. (1999). Paternal care by genetic fathers and stepfathers I: Reports from Albuquerque men. Evolution and Human Behavior, 20, 405–431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. M. (1994). The evolution of desire: Strategies of human mating. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. M. (2000). The dangerous passion: Why jealousy is as necessary as love and sex. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. M., & Shackelford, T. K. (1997). From vigilance to violence: Mate retention tactics in married couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 346–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, S. S., & Cauffman, E. (1999). Sexual betrayal among late adolescents: Perspectives of the perpetrator and the aggrieved. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 28, 235–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fenigstein, A., & Preston, M. (2007). The desired number of sexual partners as a function of gender, sexual risks, and the meaning of “ideal”. Journal of Sex Research, 44, 89–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gangestad, S. W., & Thornhill, R. (1997). The evolutionary psychology of extrapair sex: The role of fluctuating asymmetry. Evolution and Human Behavior, 18, 69–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., & Garver, C. E. (2002). Changes in women’s sexual interests and their partners’ mate-retention tactics across the menstrual cycle: Evidence for shifting conflicts of interest. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 269, 975–982.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., & Garver, C. E. (2005). Adaptations to ovulation. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 344–371). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. C. (1998). Male, female: The evolution of human sex differences. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hansen, G. L. (1985). Dating jealousy among college students. Sex Roles, 12, 713–721.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, C. R., & Christenfeld, N. (1996). Jealousy and rational responses to infidelity across gender and culture. Psychological Science, 7, 378–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haselton, M. G., & Buss, D. M. (2000). Error management theory: A new perspective on biases in cross-sex mind reading. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 81–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haselton, M. G., & Gangestad, S. W. (2006). Conditional expression of women’s desires and men’s mate guarding across the ovulatory cycle. Hormones and Behavior, 49, 509–518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lenoir, C. D., Adler, N. E., Borzekowski, D. L. G., Tschann, J. M., & Ellen, J. M. (2006). What you don’t know can hurt you: Perceptions of sex-partner concurrency and partner-reported behavior. Journal of Adolescent Health, 38, 179–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBurney, D. H., Zapp, D. J., & Streeter, S. A. (2005). Preferred number of sexual partners: Tails of distributions and tales of mating systems. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 271–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neale, M. C., Boker, S. M., Xie, G., & Maes, H. H. (2003). Mx: Statistical modeling (6th ed.). Richmond: Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Commonwealth University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaeffer, N. C. (2000). Asking questions about threatening topics: A selective overview. In N. Schwarz, & S. Sudman (Eds.), Autobiographical memory and the validity of retrospective reports (pp. 141–160). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, D. P. (2003). Universal sex differences in the desire for sexual variety: Tests from 52 nations, 6 continents, and 13 islands. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 85–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shackelford, T. K., & Buss, D. M. (1997). Cues to infidelity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 1034–1045.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shackelford, T. K., Buss, D. M., & Weekes-Shackelford, V. A. (2003). Wife killings committed in the context of a lovers’ triangle. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 25, 137–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tourangeau, R., & Smith, T. W. (1996). Asking sensitive questions: The impact of data collection mode, question format, and question context. Public Opinion Quarterly, 60, 275–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turner, C. F., Forsyth, B. H., O’Reilly, J., Cooley, P. C., Smith, T. K., Rogers, S. M., et al. (1998). Automated self-interviewing and the survey measurement of sensitive behaviors. In M. P. Couper, R. P. Baker, J. Bethlehem, C. Z. F. Clark, J. Martin, W. L. Nicholls, & J. M. O’Reilly (Eds.), Computer-assisted survey information collection (pp. 455–473). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weisfeld, G. E. (1999). Evolutionary principles of human adolescence. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M., Daly, M., & Daniele, A. (1995). Familicide: the killing of spouse and children. Aggressive Behavior, 21, 275–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yarab, P. E., Allgeier, E. R., & Sensibaugh, C. C. (1999). Looking deeper: Extradyadic behaviors, jealousy, and perceived unfaithfulness in hypothetical dating relationships. Personal Relationships, 6, 305–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

PWA was supported by a National Research Service Award from the National Institutes of Health, P32 MH-20030 (PI: Michael C. Neale). Rosalind Arden, Judith Easton, Todd Shackelford, Andy Thomson, Tina Wagers, and two anonymous reviews provided comments. Chuck Gardner provided statistical advice.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paul W. Andrews.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Andrews, P.W., Gangestad, S.W., Miller, G.F. et al. Sex Differences in Detecting Sexual Infidelity. Hum Nat 19, 347–373 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-008-9051-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-008-9051-3

Keywords

Navigation