Human Nature

, Volume 19, Issue 2, pp 119–137 | Cite as

Five Misunderstandings About Cultural Evolution

Article

Abstract

Recent debates about memetics have revealed some widespread misunderstandings about Darwinian approaches to cultural evolution. Drawing from these debates, this paper disputes five common claims: (1) mental representations are rarely discrete, and therefore models that assume discrete, gene-like particles (i.e., replicators) are useless; (2) replicators are necessary for cumulative, adaptive evolution; (3) content-dependent psychological biases are the only important processes that affect the spread of cultural representations; (4) the “cultural fitness” of a mental representation can be inferred from its successful transmission; and (5) selective forces only matter if the sources of variation are random. We close by sketching the outlines of a unified evolutionary science of culture.

Keywords

Dual inheritance theory Memes Cultural evolution Epidemiology of representations Cultural transmission Replicators 

References

  1. Atran, S. (1998). Folk biology and the anthropology of science: cognitive universals and cultural particulars. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 547–609.Google Scholar
  2. Atran, S. (2001). The trouble with memes: inference versus imitation in cultural creation. Human Nature, 12, 351–381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Atran, S. (2002). The religious landscape. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  4. Aunger, R. (2002). The electric meme: A new theory of how we think. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
  5. Basalla, G. (1988). The evolution of technology. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  6. Baum, W. M. (2005). Understanding behaviorism: Behavior, culture, evolution. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  7. Bentley, R. A., et al. (2007). Regular rates of popular culture change reflect random copying. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 151–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Blackmore, S. (1999). The meme machine. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  9. Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  10. Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1987). The evolution of ethnic markers. Cultural Anthropology, 2, 27–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1992). Punishment allows the evolution of cooperation (or anything else) in sizable groups. Ethology & Sociobiology, 13, 171–195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1995). Why does culture increase adaptability? Ethology & Sociobiology, 16, 125–143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1996). Why culture is common, but cultural evolution is rare. Proceedings of the British Academy, 88, 77–93.Google Scholar
  14. Boyer, P. (1994). The naturalness of religious ideas. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  15. Boyer, P. (1999). Cognitive tracks of cultural inheritance: how evolved intuitive ontology governs cultural transmission. American Anthropologist, 100, 876–889.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Boyer, P. (2001). Religion explained: The evolutionary origins of religious thought. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  17. Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., & Feldman, M. (1981). Cultural transmission and evolution. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  18. Claidiere, N., & Sperber, D. (2007). The role of attraction in cultural evolution. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 7, 89–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Collard, M., Shennan, S. J., & Tehrani, J. J. (2006). Branching, blending, and the evolution of cultural similarities and differences among human populations. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27, 168–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Conot, R. E. (1979). A streak of luck. New York: Seaview Books (distributed by Simon and Schuster).Google Scholar
  21. Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. Oxford: Oxford Unversity Press.Google Scholar
  22. Dawkins, R. (1982). The extended phenotype. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  23. Dennett, D. (1995). Darwin’s dangerous idea. London: Penguin Press.Google Scholar
  24. Diamond, J. M. (1997). Guns, germs, and steel: The fates of human societies. New York: W.W. Norton & Co.Google Scholar
  25. Durham, W. H. (1991). Coevolution: Genes, culture, and human diversity. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  26. Eerkens, J. W., & Lipo, C. P. (2005). Cultural transmission, copying errors, and the generation of variation in material culture and the archaeological record. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 24, 316–334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Grant, P., & Grant, R. (1986). Ecology and evolution of Darwin’s finches. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  28. Hager, T. (2007). The demon under the microscope. New York: Three Rivers Press.Google Scholar
  29. Henrich, J. (2001). Cultural transmission and the diffusion of innovations: adoption dynamics indicate that biased cultural transmission is the predominate force in behavioral change and much of sociocultural evolution. American Anthropologist, 103, 992–1013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Henrich, J. (2004). Demography and cultural evolution: why adaptive cultural processes produced maladaptive losses in Tasmania. American Antiquity, 69, 197–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Henrich, J. (2007). The evolution of costly displays, cooperation, and religion: Inferentially potent displays and their implications for cultural evolution. Economics and Evolution working paper series. Evolutionary Economics Group, MPI Jena, Germany. <Available at https://papers.econ.mpg.de/evo/discussionpapers/2007–21.pdf>
  32. Henrich, J., & Boyd, R. (1998). The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence of between-group differences. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19, 215–242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Henrich, J., & Boyd, R. (2001). Why people punish defectors: weak conformist transmission can stabilize costly enforcement of norms in cooperative dilemmas. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 208, 79–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Henrich, J., & Boyd, R. (2002). On modeling cultural evolution: why replicators are not necessary for cultural evolution. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 2(2), 87–112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Henrich, J, & Boyd, R. (In press). Division of labor, economic specialization, and the evolution of social stratification. Current Anthropology, in press.Google Scholar
  36. Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. (2001). The evolution of prestige: freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 165–196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Henrich, J., & McElreath, R. (2006). Dual inheritance theory: The evolution of human cultural capacities and cultural evolution. In R. Dunbar, & L. Barrett (Eds.) Oxford Handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 555–570). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  38. Henrich, J. et al. (Eds.) (2004). Foundations of human sociality: Economic experiments and ethnographic evidence from fifteen small-scale societies. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  39. Henrich, N. S., & Henrich, J. (2007). Why humans cooperate: A cultural and evolutionary explanation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  40. Johnson, A. W., & Price-Williams, D. R. (1996). Oedipus ubiquitous: The family complex in world folk literature. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  41. Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, J., & Feldman, M. W. (2000). Niche construction, biological evolution, and cultural change. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 131–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Leslie, A. M. (1994). ToMM, ToBY, and agency: Core architecture and domain specificity. In L. A. Hirschfeld, & S. A. Gelman (Eds.) Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 119–148). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  43. Lipo, C. P. (Ed.) (2006). Mapping our ancestors. Piscataway, NJ: Aldine Transaction.Google Scholar
  44. Martindale, C. (1975). Romantic progression: The psychology of literary history. Halsted: Washington Hemisphere.Google Scholar
  45. McElreath, R. (2003). Reputation and the evolution of conflict. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 220, 345–357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. McElreath, R., & Boyd, R. (2007). Modeling the evolution of social behavior. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  47. McElreath, R., & Strimling, P. (2008). When natural selection favors imitation of parents. Current Anthropology, in press.Google Scholar
  48. McElreath, R., Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2003). Shared norms and the evolution of ethnic markers. Current Anthropology, 44, 122–129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. McElreath, R., et al. (2005). Applying evolutionary models to the laboratory study of social learning. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 483–508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Mesoudi, A. (2008). The cultural transmission of Great Basin projectile technology: an experimental simulation. American Antiquity, 73, 3–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Mesoudi, A., & Laland, K. N. (2007). Culturally transmitted paternity beliefs and the evolution of human mating behaviour. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. B: Biological Sciences, 274, 1273–1278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., & Laland, K. N. (2004). Perspective: is human cultural evolution Darwinian? Evidence reviewed from the perspective of The Origin of Species. Evolution, 58, 1–11.Google Scholar
  53. Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., & Laland, K. N. (2006a). A science of culture: clarifications and extensions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 366–383.Google Scholar
  54. Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., & Laland, K. N. (2006b). Towards a unified science of cultural evolution. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 329–347.Google Scholar
  55. Meyers, M. A. (2007). Happy accidents: Serendipity in modern medical breakthroughs. New York: Arcade.Google Scholar
  56. Offerman, T., & Sonnemans, J. (1998). Learning by experience and learning by imitating others. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 34, 559–575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Panchanathan, K., & Boyd, R. (2004). Indirect reciprocity can stabilize cooperation without the second-order free rider problem. Nature, 432, 499–502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
  59. Richerson, P., & Boyd, R. (1998). The evolution of ultrasociality. In I. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, & F. K. Salter (Eds.) Indoctrinability, ideology and warfare (pp. 71–96). New York: Berghahn Books.Google Scholar
  60. Richerson, P., & Boyd, R. (2000). Complex societies: the evolutionary dynamics of a crude superorganism. Human Nature, 10, 253–289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Richerson, P., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  62. Rogers, A. (1989). Does biology constrain culture? American Anthropologist, 90, 819–831.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Rogers, E. M. (1995). Diffusion of innovations. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
  64. Rosenthal, T. L., & Zimmerman, B. J. (1978). Social learning and cognition. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  65. Shennan, S. (2001). Demography and cultural innovation: a model and its implications for the emergence of modern human culture. Cambridge Archaeology Journal, 11, 5–16.Google Scholar
  66. Shennan, S. (2003). Genes, memes, and human history: Darwinian archaeology and cultural evolution. London: Thames & Hudson.Google Scholar
  67. Shennan, S. (in press). Pattern and process in cultural evolution. Berkeley: University of California.Google Scholar
  68. Sneader, W. (2005). Drug discovery: A history. Chichester, England, and Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
  69. Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining culture: A naturalistic approach. Oxford, UK, and Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  70. Taylor, G. (1996). Cultural selection. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  71. Tomasello, M. (1996). Do apes ape? In C. M. Heyes, & B. G. Galef Jr. (Eds.) Social learning in animals: The roots of culture (pp. 319–346). San Diego: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.) The adapted mind (pp. 19–136). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  73. Whiten, A. (2000). Primate culture and social learning. Cognitive Science, 24(3), 477–508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Williams, T. (1987). The history of invention. New York: Facts on File.Google Scholar
  75. Wrangham, R. W., et al. (1999). The raw and the stolen: cooking and the ecology of human origins. Current Anthropology, 40, 567–594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science + Business Media, LLC 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  • Joseph Henrich
    • 1
  • Robert Boyd
    • 2
  • Peter J. Richerson
    • 3
  1. 1.Departments of Psychology and EconomicsUniversity of British ColumbiaVancouverCanada
  2. 2.Department of AnthropologyUniversity of California-Los AngelesLos AngelesUSA
  3. 3.Department of Environmental Science and PolicyUniversity of California-DavisDavisUSA

Personalised recommendations