Abstract
Despite being a widely researched topic, there is a lack of consensus across studies in how fear of crime is measured. Scholars have often used perceived safety or risk as proxies for fear of crime, without acknowledging that these may be distinct constructs (which warrant their own study), and thus may not be adequate measures of emotional fear of crime. The current study examines the prevalence, frequency, and magnitude of “fear” estimated by a measure of emotional fear compared to measures of perceived safety and risk. In addition to comparing these measures, and providing a replication of work that to date has only been conducted in the UK, the current study specifically explores the implications of using different proxies of fear of crime for testing the broken windows thesis.
Similar content being viewed by others
Notes
However, Ferraro (1995) notes later that the effects of age on fear are rendered non-significant when controlling for other factors (p. 70–71).
As part of that research, these surveys were identified through database searches for studies of fear of crime and victimization. To be eligible for inclusion, the studies must have tested the relationship between victimization and fear of crime (i.e. at least reported the correlation between the two). However, that did not have to be the main focus of the study, it was simply necessary for the published results to statistically examine the relationship between fear and victimization in some manner. Several broad search terms were used so that studies using any measures of “fear” would be found. For instance, one keyword combination searched for studies including the word “survey” and “victim” (or “victimization”). Thus the results included surveys that used measures of perceived safety, perceived risk, and worry about crime/victimization as the search strategy did not require use of the term “fear of crime.” For full details, see Yang and Hinkle (2012).
All interviewers were trained by an experienced survey researcher prior to the beginning of the project. Additionally, the survey was first piloted in a city removed from the study. All interviewers were required to satisfactorily complete pilot shifts before calling respondents in the study sample.
Specifically, the database used was Powerfinder—a comprehensive reverse directory of phone numbers containing address information collected and sold by INFOUSA. For Redlands and Colton the PowerFinder database was supplemented with a list of phone numbers on the study blocks provided by each city’s water department. It is important to note that like all recent telephone survey research, some households could be excluded from possible inclusion due to lacking landlines as cell phone numbers are not included in such reverse directory databases. Unlisted numbers would be excluded as well. However, the threat of bias here is no greater than in other recent phone survey research on fear of crime (see footnote 9), and thus should not pose a threat in making comparisons to such work.
For each street block in the study, all of the households/businesses present in the database were entered into an SPSS file (one file per street block). A random number variable was created in each file, and the cases were then sorted in ascending order on this variable. Numbers were then called until all were exhausted or the target of ten completed surveys was reached. The limit of ten was set as study blocks had to have a minimum of only seven households/businesses present in the telephone database to be eligible for inclusion in the study. Knowing that there would be some streets where only three to five surveys were likely to be completed, it was decided to set the maximum for one street block to ten to avoid oversampling certain blocks and biasing the data. Furthermore, power analyses showed high levels of statistical power with only 3.5 respondents per block (or complete details on the survey methodology, see Weisburd, Hinkle, Famega, & Ready, 2012).
Every attempt was made to obtain completed surveys from valid phone numbers. Specifically, an address/phone number was only dropped from the study under three conditions. First, if the person was irate or threatening on the first contact. Second, if the person verbally refused to take the survey on two separate calls (at least 2 days apart). Third, if there was no answer after five attempts on different days/times.
Business owners/managers were chosen as it was presumed that they would be more invested in the street than other workers. In other words, they were assumed to be more similar to people who lived on the blocks compared to workers and were chosen to keep the sampling logic consistent. The goal was to survey people who would presumably be familiar with the social and physical environment of that street block, and thus residents and owners/managers were deemed the best fits with the reasoning being that those groups would presumably be more attached to/involved with the area than a general employee.
The cooperation rate excludes cases that were coded as chronic no answer/busy/answering machine (n = 307) and cases where there was a language (not an English or Spanish speaker) or cognitive barrier (N = 59) from the denominator.
Specifically, a study using random digit dialing in the state of Kentucky had a response rate of 27.5 % (Rader, May, & Goodrum, 2007), a study of fear in Dallas neighborhoods had a response rate of 33.4 % (Ferguson & Mindel, 2007), and a study by Xu, Fiedler, and Flaming (2005) achieved a response rate of 60 %, and a study in Philadelphia had a response rate of 77 % (Wyant, 2008). It is worth noting that the Xu et al. data were collected by a police department (and respondents may be less likely to refuse a survey collected directly by the police) and the Wyant study involved a $10 monetary reward for respondents to encourage participation.
At the beginning of the survey the respondents’ street blocks were clearly defined to them as the street they lived on between the two intersections that defined the boundaries of the street block for the study.
The perceived social disorder measure includes fist fights, people loitering or being disorderly, public drinking, drunk or high in public, panhandlers, vandalism, people making too much noise late at night/early morning, gambling in the street, drug sales, and prostitution.
The perceived physical disorder measure includes broken windows, graffiti, abandoned or boarded-up buildings, vacant lots, abandoned cars, litter, street or sidewalks in need of repair, and areas in need of better lighting.
The crime scale includes all reports of the following completed or attempted offenses: arson, assault/battery, auto theft, burglary, carjacking, grand theft, petty theft, rape, and robbery.
As perceived risk scores were averaged across items, the scores were rounded to the nearest whole number to categorize respondents into one of the four response option categories for this table. For example, an average score between 1 and 1.49 was rounded down to 1 (very unlikely), a score between 1.5 and 2.49 was rounded up/down to 2 (unlikely) and so on.
The ordinal regression models use the logit link function. Table 7 provides the test of parallel lines results and the non-significant findings support use of the logit link function.
Analyses were also conducted using logistic regressions for the perceived risk and safety measures (by collapsing those variables into two levels) to allow for easier comparison to the fear model presented in Table 6. Results were substantively similar with one exception; gender was no longer a significant predictor of perceived safety. This was due to a reduction in variation from collapsing the ordinal measure and losing the gender difference in “very unsafe” responses compared to “somewhat unsafe.” Therefore, it was decided to present the ordinal regression models as they represent the perceived safety and risk measures as they were designed, which is important given the focus of this paper on comparing alternative measurement schemes.
In the current study the “fear” items were all asked very early in the survey, before any questions related to perceived crime/disorder or victimization experiences. The victimization questions were asked near the end of the survey. The full survey instrument is too lengthy to include in this article, but is available on the NCJRS website as an appendix in the final grant report to the National Institute of Justice (Weisburd et al., 2012) at the following URL: https://www.ncjrs.gov/pdffiles1/nij/grants/239971.pdf.
References
Bratton, W. J., & Knobler, P. (1998). Turnaround: How America’s top cop reversed the crime epidemic. New York: Random House.
Ditton, J., Bannister, J., Gilchrist, E., & Farrall, S. (1999a). Attitudes toward crime, victimization and the police in Scotland: A comparison of white and ethnic minority views. Edinburgh: Scottish Office Central Research Unit Report.
Ditton, J., Bannister, J., Gilchrist, E., & Farrall, S. (1999b). Afraid or angry? Recalibrating the ‘fear’ of crime. International Review of Victimology, 6(2), 83–99.
Farrall, S. (2004). Revisiting crime surveys: Emotional responses without emotions? International Journal of Social Research Methodology, 7(2), 157–171.
Farrall, S., Bannister, J., Ditton, J., & Gilchrist, E. (1997). Questioning the measurement of the ‘fear of crime.’. British Journal of Criminology, 37(4), 658–679.
Farrall, S., & Ditton, J. (1999). Improving the measurement of attitudinal responses: An example from a crime survey. International Journal of Social Research Methodology, 2(1), 55–68.
Farrall, S., & Gadd, D. (2004). Research note: The frequency of the fear of crime. British Journal of Criminology, 44, 127–132.
Farrall, S., & Gadd, D., (2004b). Fear today, gone tomorrow: Do surveys overstate fear levels? Retrieved from http://www.istat.it/istat/eventi/perunasocieta/relazioni/Farral_abs.pdf.
Farrall, S., Jackson, J., & Gray, E. (2009). Social order and the fear of crime in contemporary times. New York: Oxford University Press.
Ferguson, K. M., & Mindel, C. H. (2007). Modeling fear of crime in Dallas neighborhoods: A test of social capital theory. Crime and Delinquency, 53(2), 322–350.
Ferraro, K. F. (1995). Fear of crime: Interpreting victimization risk. Albany: State University of New York Press.
Ferraro, K. F., & LaGrange, R. L. (1987). The measurement of fear of crime. Sociological Inquiry, 57(1), 70–101.
Gabriel, U., & Greve, W. (2003). The psychology of fear of crime: Conceptual and methodological perspectives. British Journal of Criminology, 43, 600–614.
Garofalo, J. (1979). Victimization and the fear of crime. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 16, 80–97.
Garofalo, J., & Laub, J. H. (1978). The fear of crime: Broadening our perspective. Victimology, 3(3–4), 242–253.
Giuliani, R. W., & Kurson, K. (2002). Leadership. New York: Hyperion.
Gray, E., Jackson, J., & Farrall, S. (2008). Reassessing the fear of crime. European Journal of Criminology, 5(3), 1477–3708.
Gray, E., Jackson, J., & Farrall, S. (2011). Feelings and functions in the fear of crime: Applying a new approach to victimization insecurity. British Journal of Criminology, 51, 75–94.
Hinkle, J. C. (2013). The relationship between disorder, perceived risk and collective efficacy: A look into the indirect pathways of the broken windows thesis. Criminal Justice Studies, 4, 408–432.
Hinkle, J. C., Weisburd, D., Famega, C., & Ready, J. (2014). The problem is not just sample size: The consequences of low base rates in policing experiments in smaller cities. Evaluation Review. doi:10.1177/0193841X13519799.
Hinkle, J. C., & Yang, S. (2014). A new look into broken windows: What shapes individuals’ perceptions of social disorder? Journal of Criminal Justice, 42, 26–35.
Hipp, J. R. (2010). Resident perceptions of crime and disorder: How much is “bias”, and how much is social environment differences? Criminology, 48(2), 475–508.
Hough, M. (2004). Worry about crime: Mental events or mental states? International Journal of Social Research Methodology, 7(2), 173–176.
Hunter, A. (1978). Symbols of incivility: Social disorder and fear of crime in urban neighborhoods. Dallas: Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Society of Criminology.
Innes, M. (2004). Signal crimes and signal disorders: Notes on deviance as communicative action. The British Journal of Sociology, 55(3), 335–355.
Jackson, J. (2009). A psychological perspective on vulnerability in the fear of crime. Psychology, Crime and Law, 15(4), 365–390.
Jackson, J., & Gray, E. (2010). Functional fear and public insecurities about crime. British Journal of Criminology, 50, 1–22.
Karmon, A. (2000). New York murder mystery: The true story behind the crime crash of the 1990s. New York: New York University Press.
Kelling, G. L., & Coles, C. (1996). Fixing broken windows: Restoring order and reducing crime in American cities. New York: Free Press.
Kelling, G. L., & Sousa, W. H. (2001). Do police matter? An analysis of the impact of New York City’s police reforms (Civic Report No., 22). New York: Manhattan Institute for Policy Research.
Killias, M., & Clerici, C. (2000). Different measures of vulnerability in their relation to different dimensions of fear of crime. British Journal of Criminology, 40, 437–450.
Lewis, D. A., & Salem, G. (1986). Fear of crime: Incivility and the production of a social problem. New Brunswick: Transaction Books.
Lum, C., Koper, C. S., & Telep, C. W. (2011). The evidence-based policing matrix. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 7(1), 3–26.
Maple, J., & Mitchell, C. (1999). The crime fighter: How you can make your community crime free. New York: Broadway.
Markon, K. E., Chmielewsky, M., & Miller, C. J. (2001). The reliability and validity of discrete and continuous measures of psychopatholgy: A quantitative review. Psychological Bulletin, 137(5), 856–879.
Pate, A. M., Skogan, W. G., Wycoff, M. A., & Sherman, L. W. (1985a). Reducing the “Signs of Crime:” The Newark Experience. Washington: The Police Foundation.
Pate, A. M., Skogan, W. G., Wycoff, M. A., & Sherman, L. W. (1985b). Coordinated Community Policing: The Newark Experience. The Newark Experience. Washington: The Police Foundation.
Rader, N. E., May, D. C., & Goodrum, S. (2007). An empirical assessment of the “threat of victimization:” Considering fear of crime, perceived risk, avoidance, and defensive behaviors. Sociological Spectrum, 27(5), 475–505.
Rountree, P. W., & Land, K. C. (1996). Perceived risk versus fear of crime: Empirical evidence of conceptually distinct reactions in survey data. Social Forces, 74(4), 1353–1376.
Sampson, R. J., & Raudenbush, S. W. (1999). Systematic social observation of public spaces: A new look at disorder in urban neighborhoods. American Journal of Sociology, 105(3), 603–651.
Silverman, E. B. (1999). NYPD battles crime: Innovative strategies in policing. Boston: Northeastern University Press.
Skogan, W. G. (1990). Disorder and decline: Crime and the spiral of decline in American neighborhoods. New York: Free Press.
St. Jean, P. K. B. (2007). Pockets of crime: Broken windows, collective efficacy, and the criminal point of view. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Taylor, R. B. (2001). Breaking away from broken windows: Baltimore neighborhoods and the nationwide fight against crime, grime, gear, and decline. Boulder: Westview Press.
U.S. Census Bureau. (2010). United States Census 2010. Retrieved from https://www.census.gov/2010census/.
Warr, M. (1984). Fear of victimization: Why are women and the elderly more afraid? Social Science Quarterly, 65(3), 681–702.
Warr, M. (2000). Fear of crime in the United States: Avenues for policy and research. In Measurement and Analysis of Crime and Justice, Volume 4 (pp. 451–489). Washington: US Department of Justice, Office of Justice Programs.
Weisburd, D., Hinkle, J. C., Famega, C., & Ready, J. (2011). The possible “backfire” effects of hot Spots policing: An experimental assessment of impacts on legitimacy, fear and collective efficacy. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 7, 297–320.
Weisburd, D., Hinkle, J. C., Famega, C., & Ready, J. (2012). Legitimacy, fear and collective efficacy in crime hot spots: Assessing the impacts of broken windows policing strategies on citizen attitudes. Washington: Department of Justice: National Institute of Justice. Grant No. 2007-IJ-CX-0047.
Wilson, J. Q. (1975). Thinking about crime. New York: Basic Books.
Wilson, J. Q., & Kelling, G. L. (1982). Broken windows: The police and neighborhood safety. Atlantic Monthly, 211, 29–38.
Wyant, B. R. (2008). Multilevel impacts of perceived incivilities and perceptions of crime risk on fear of crime: Isolating endogenous impacts. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 45(1), 29–64.
Xu, Y., Fiedler, M. L., & Flaming, K. H. (2005). Discovering the impact of community policing: The broken windows thesis, collective efficacy and citizens’ judgment. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 42(2), 147–186.
Yang, S. (2007). Causal or merely co-existing: A longitudinal study of disorder and violence at places (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). College Park: University of Maryland.
Yang, S. (2010). Assessing the spatial-temporal relationship between disorder and violence. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 26(1), 139–163.
Yang, S., & Hinkle, J. C. (2012). Issues in survey design–Using surveys of victimization and fear of crime as examples. In L. Gideon (Ed.), The Handbook of Survey Methodology in Social Sciences (pp. 443–462). New York: Springer.
Yang, S., & Wyckoff, L. A. (2010). Perceptions of safety and victimization: Does survey construction affect perceptions? Journal of Experimental Criminology, 6(3), 293–323.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported in part by grant no. 2007-IJ-CX-0047 from the National Institute of Justice.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Hinkle, J.C. Emotional Fear of Crime vs. Perceived Safety and Risk: Implications for Measuring “Fear” and Testing the Broken Windows Thesis. Am J Crim Just 40, 147–168 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12103-014-9243-9
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12103-014-9243-9