Advertisement

Chemosensory Perception

, Volume 6, Issue 4, pp 198–210 | Cite as

The Relationship Between Psychopathy and Olfactory Tasks Sensitive to Orbitofrontal Cortex Function in a Non-criminal Student Sample

  • Travis M. Bettison
  • Mehmet K. MahmutEmail author
  • Richard J. Stevenson
Article

Abstract

This study investigated the relationship between psychopathy and tests presumed sensitive to orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) function in a non-criminal student sample. While converging lines of evidence indicate OFC-associated dysfunction in criminal psychopaths, few studies have investigated whether non-criminal psychopaths manifest similar deficits. Psychopathic traits were indexed using the Self-Report Psychopathy scale and the “Sniffin' Sticks” and Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) were employed as neuropsychological measures of OFC function. The results showed higher degrees of psychopathy were significantly associated with poorer olfactory discriminative ability and poorer IGT performance. The discussion focuses on what these findings contribute to the understanding of the psychopathy and OFC relationship, suggesting the degree of OFC-associated dysfunction may be one differentiating factor between criminal and non-criminal psychopaths.

Keywords

Psychopathy Olfaction Orbitofrontal cortex Non-criminal 

Notes

Compliance with Ethics Requirements

Conflict of Interest

Travis M. Bettison declares that he has no conflict of interest.

Mehmet K. Mahmut declares that he has no conflict of interest.

Richard J. Stevenson declares that he has no conflict of interest.

All procedures followed were in accordance with the ethical standards of the responsible committee on human experimentation (institutional and national) and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008 (5). Informed consent was obtained from all patients for being included in the study.

References

  1. Acheson SK (2005) Review of the Hare Psychopathy Checklist—Revised. In Spies RA, Plake BS (Eds) The Sixteenth Mental Measurements Yearbook, 2nd ed. Buros Institute of Mental Measurements Lincoln, pp. 429–431Google Scholar
  2. Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW (1994) Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 50:7–15CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Birbaumer N, Veit R, Lotze M, Erb M, Hermann C, Grodd W, Flor H (2005) Deficient fear conditioning in psychopathy: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch Gen Psychiatr 62(7):799–805CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Blair RJR (2004) The roles of orbital frontal cortex in the modulation of antisocial behaviour. Brain Cogn 55:198–208CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Blair RJR (2006) Subcortical brain systems in psychopathy: the amygdala and associated structures. In: Patrick CJ (ed) Handbook of psychopathy. Guilford, New York, pp 459–478Google Scholar
  6. Blair RJR, Mitchell DGV, Richell RA, Kelly S, Leonard A, Newman C, Scott SK (2002) Turning a deaf ear to fear: impaired recognition of vocal affect in psychopathic individuals. J Abnorm Psychol 111(4):682–686CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Blair KS, Newman C, Mitchell DGV, Richell RA, Leonard A, Morton J, Blair RJR (2006) Differentiating among prefrontal substrates in psychopathy: neuropsychological test findings. Neuropsychology 20(2):153–165CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Borod JC (1992) Interhemipsheric and intrahemispheric control of emotion: a focus on unilateral brain damage. J Consult Clin Psychol 60:339–348CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Cain WS, Gent JF, Goodspeed RB, Leonard G (1988) Evaluation of olfactory dysfunction in the Connecticut Chemosensory Clinical Research Center (CCCRC). Laryngoscope 98:83–88CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Clark LA (2007) Assessment and diagnosis of personality disorder: perennial issues and an emerging reconceptualization. Annu Rev Psychol 58:227–257CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Cooke DJ, Michie C, Hart SD, Clark DA (2004) Reconstructing psychopathy: clarifying the significance of antisocial and socially deviant behavior in the diagnosis of psychopathic personality disorder. J Pers Disord 18:337–357CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dade LA, Zatorre RJ, Jones–Gotman M (2002) Olfactory learning convergent findings from lesion and brain imaging studies. Brain 125:86–101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Damasio AR (1994) Descartes' error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. Avon, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  14. De Araujo I, Rolls ET, Velazco M, Margot C, Cayeux I (2005) Cognitive modulation of olfactory processing. Neuron 46:671–679CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Doty RL, Shaman P, Dann M (1984) Development of the university of Pennsylvania smell identification test: a standardized microencapsulated test of olfactory function. Physiol Behav 32(3):489–502CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Doty RL, Marcus A, Lee WW (1996) Development of the 12-item cross-cultural smell identification test (CC-SIT). Laryngoscope 106:353–356CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Gorenstein EE (1982) Frontal lobe functions in psychopaths. J Abnorm Psychol 95:368–379CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gottfriend JA, O'Doherty J, Dolan RJ (2002) Appetitive and aversive olfactory learning in humans studied using event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci 22(24):10829–10837Google Scholar
  19. Goudriaan AE, Oosterlaan J, de Beurs E, van den Brink W (2006) Psychophysiological determinants and concomitants of deficient decision making in pathological gamblers. Drug Alcohol Depend 84(3):231–239CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Hall JR, Benning SD (2006) The “successful” psychopath: adaptive and subclinical manifestations of psychopath in the general population. In: Patrick CJ (ed) Handbook of psychopathy. Guilford, New York, pp 459–478Google Scholar
  21. Hare RD (1991) The Hare psychopathy checklist–revised. Multi–Health Systems, TorontoGoogle Scholar
  22. Hare RD (1998) Psychopathy, affect, and behavior. In: Cooke DJ, Forth AR, Hare RD (eds) Psychopathy: theory, research, and implications for society. Kluwer, Dordrecht, pp 105–138CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hare RD (2003) The Hare psychopathy checklist—revised 2nd edition. Multi-Health Systems, TorontoGoogle Scholar
  24. Hare RD, Neumann CS (2008) Psychopathy as a clinical and empirical construct. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 4:217–247CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hare RD, Neumann CS (2010) The role of antisociality in the psychopathy construct: comment on Skeem and Cooke (2010). Psychol Assess 22:446–454CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Harpur TJ, Hare RD, Hakistian AR (1989) Two-factor conceptualization of psychopathy: construct validity and assessment implications. J Consult Clin Psychol 1:6–17Google Scholar
  27. Harris GT, Rice ME, Quinsey VL (1994) Psychopathy as a taxon: evidence that psychopaths are a discreet class. J Consult Clin Psychol 62:387–397CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Hummel T, Sekinger B, Wolf SR, Pauli E, Kobal G (1997) ‘Sniffin' Sticks’: olfactory performance assessed by the combined testing of odor identification, odor discrimination and olfactory threshold. Chem Senses 22:39–52CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Hummel T, Kobal G, Gudziol H, Mackay-Sim A (2007) Normative data for the Sniffin' Sticks including test of odor identification, odor discrimination, and olfactory thresholds: an upgrade based on a group of more than 3,000 subjects. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol 264:237–243CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Ishikawa SS, Raine A, Lencz T, Bihrle S, Lacasse L (2001) Autonomic stress reactivity and executive functions in successful and unsuccessful criminal psychopaths from the community. J Abnorm Psychol 110(3):423–432CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Kennealy PJ, Hicks BM, Patrick CJ (2007) Validity of the psychopathy checklist—revised with female offenders. Assessment 14:323–340CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. LaPierre D, Braun CMJ, Hodgins S (1995) Ventral frontal deficits in psychopathy: neuropsychological test findings. Neuropsychologia 33(2):139–151CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Lidberg L, Levander SE, Schalling D, Lidberg Y (1978) Necker cube reversals, arousal and psychopathy. Brit J Soc Clin Psychol 17(4):355–361CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Lösel F, Schmucker M (2004) Psychopathy, risk-taking and attention: a differentiated test of the somatic marker hypothesis. J Abnorm Psychol 113(4):522–529CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Mahmut MK, Stevenson RJ (2012) Olfactory abilities and psychopathy: higher psychopathy scores are associated with poorer odor discrimination and identification. Chemosens Percept 5:300–307CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Mahmut MK, Homewood J, Stevenson RJ (2008) The characteristics of non-criminals with high psychopathy traits: are they similar to criminal psychopaths? J Res Pers 42:679–692CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Mahmut MK, Menictas C, Stevenson RJ, Homewood J (2011) Validating the factor structure of the self-report psychopathy scale in a community sample. Psychol Assess 23(3):670–678CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Mehrabian A, Epstein N (1972) A measure of emotional empathy. J Pers 40:525–543CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Meloy JR (1988) The psychopathic mind. Jason Aronson, NorthvaleGoogle Scholar
  40. Miranda R, Mackillop J, Meyerson LA, Justus A, Lovallo WR (2009) Influence of antisocial and psychopathic traits on decision-making biases in alcoholics. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 33:817–825CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Mitchell DGV, Colledge E, Leonard A, Blair RJR (2002) Risky decisions and response reversal: is there evidence of orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in psychopathic individuals? Neuropsychologia 40(12):2013–2022CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Mitchell DGV, Fine C, Richell RA, Newman C, Lumsden J, Blair KS, Blair RJR (2006) Instrumental learning and relearning in individuals with psychopathy and in patients with lesions involving the amygdala or orbitofrontal cortex. Neuropsychology 20(3):280–289CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Morgan AB, Lilienfeld SO (2000) A meta-analytic review of the relation between antisocial behaviour and neuropsychological measures of executive function. Clin Psychol Rev 20:113–136CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Mullins–Sweatt SN, Glover NG, Derefinko KJ, Miller JD, Widiger TA (2010) The search for the successful psychopath. J Res Pers 44:554–558CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Nelson HE, Willison JR (1991) The Revised National Adult Reading Test—test manual. NFER-Nelson, WindsorGoogle Scholar
  46. Pollatos O, Albrecht J, Kopietz R, Linn J, Schoepf V, Kleemann AM, Schreder T et al (2007) Reduced olfactory sensitivity in subjects with depressive symptoms. J Affect Disord 102(1–3):101–108CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Raine A (2008) From genes to brain to antisocial behaviour. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 17(5):323–328CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Rogers RD (2006) The functional architecture of the frontal lobes: implications for research with psychopathic offenders. In: Patrick CJ (ed) Handbook of psychopathy. Guilford, New York, pp 313–333Google Scholar
  49. Rolls ET (2000) The orbitofrontal cortex and reward. Cereb Cortex 10:284–294CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Roussy S, Toupin J (2000) Behavioral inhibition deficits in juvenile psychopaths. Aggress Behav 26:413–424CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Royet JP, Hudry J, Zald DH, Godinot D, Gregoire MC, Lavenne F, Costes N, Holley A (2001) Functional neuroanatomy of different olfactory judgments. Neuroimage 13:506–519CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Rupp CI, Fleischhacker WW, Hausmann A, Mair D, Hinterhuber H, Kurz M (2004) Olfactory functioning in patients with alcohol dependence: impairments in odor judgments. Alcohol Alcohol 39(6):514–519CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Samter W, Burleson BR (1984) Cognitive and motivational influences on spontaneous comforting behavior. Hum Commun Res 11:231–260CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Schmitt WA, Brinkley CA, Newman JP (1999) Testing Damasio's somatic marker hypothesis with psychopathy individuals: risk takers or risk averse? J Abnorm Psychol 108(3):538–543CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Seubert J, Freiherr J, Frasnelli J, Hummel T, Lundstrom JN (2012) Orbitofrontal cortex and olfactory bulb volume predict distinct aspects of olfactory performance in healthy subjects. Cereb Cortex. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs230, Epub ahead of printGoogle Scholar
  56. Skeem JL, Cooke DJ (2010a) Is criminal behaviour a central component of psychopathy? Conceptual directions for resolving the debate. Psychol Assess 22(2):433–445CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Skeem JL, Cooke DJ (2010b) One measure does not a construct make: directions toward reinvigorating psychopathy research—reply to Hare and Neumann (2010). Psychol Assess 22:455–459CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Spinella M (2002) A relationship between smell identification and empathy. Int J Neurosci 112:605–612CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Widom CS (1977) A methodology for studying noninstitutionalized psychopaths. J Consult Clin Psychol 45(4):674–683CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Williams KM, Paulhus DL, Hare RD (2007) Capturing the four-factor structure of psychopathy in college students via self-report. J Pers Assess 88(2):205–219CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Wilson D, Stevenson RJ (2006) Learning to smell. John Hopkins University Press, BaltimoreGoogle Scholar
  62. Wong S (1988) Is Hares' psychopath checklist reliable without the interview? Psychol Rep 62:931–934CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  • Travis M. Bettison
    • 1
  • Mehmet K. Mahmut
    • 1
    Email author
  • Richard J. Stevenson
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyMacquarie UniversitySydneyAustralia

Personalised recommendations