Chemosensory Perception

, Volume 2, Issue 3, pp 154–160 | Cite as

Male Sexual Orientation Affects Sensitivity to Androstenone

  • Katrin Lübke
  • Sylvia Schablitzky
  • Bettina M. Pause
Article

Abstract

Androstenone has often been discussed as a human social chemosignal, as it is one of the major contributing substances to the human body odor for which humans possess specific receptors. Here, we investigated whether male sexual orientation influences the perception of androstenone. Sensitivity to androstenone, subjective ratings of intensity, pleasantness, unpleasantness, and familiarity of the odor, as well as self-reported emotional responses (valence, arousal, dominance) to the odor were assessed in 13 homosexual and 14 heterosexual men. Isovaleric acid served as a control substance. Homosexual men displayed significantly higher olfactory sensitivity to the odor of androstenone than heterosexual men (p < 0.05), but they did not differ from heterosexual men in their sensitivity to isovaleric acid (p > 0.25). Moreover, both groups did not differ in their judgments of or in their emotional response to androstenone or isovaleric acid. The current results indicate that men's sexual orientation significantly impacts the perception of androstenone but that this effect does not necessarily extend to subjective judgments.

Keywords

Androstenone Body Odor Chemosensory Communication Olfactory Sensitivity Olfactory Threshold Sexual Orientation 

References

  1. Ayabe-Kanamura S, Schicker I, Laska M, Hudson R, Distel H, Kobayakawa T, Saito S (1998) Differences in perception of everyday odors: a Japanese-German cross-cultural study. Chem Senses 23(1):31–38CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bensafi M, Brown WM, Khan R, Levenson B, Sobel N (2004) Sniffing human sex-steroid derived compounds modulates mood, memory, and autonomic nervous system function in specific behavioral contexts. Behav Brain Res 152(1):11–22Google Scholar
  3. Berglund H, Lindström P, Savic I (2006) Brain responses to putative pheromones in lesbian women. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103(21):8269–8274CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Berkey BR, Perelman-Hall T, Kurdek LA (1990) The multidimensional scale of sexuality. J Homosex 19(4):67–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Boyle J, Lundström JN, Knecht M, Jones-Gotman M, Schaal B, Hummel T (2006) On the trigeminal percept of androstenone and its implications on the rate of specific anosmia. J Neurobiol 66(13):1501–1510CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bradley MM, Lang PJ (1994) Measuring emotion: the self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 25(1):49–59CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bremner EA, Mainland JD, Khan RM, Sobel N (2003) The prevalence of androstenone anosmia. Chem Senses 28(5):423–432CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Brodie HK, Gartrell N, Doering C, Rhue T (1974) Plasma testosterone levels in heterosexual and homosexual men. Am J Psychiatry 131(1):82–83Google Scholar
  9. Brooksbank BW, Cunningham AE, Wilson DA (1969) The detection of androsta-4, 16-dien-3-one in peripheral plasma of adult men. Steroid 13(1):29–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cohen J (1965) Some statistical issues in psychological research. In: Wolman BB (ed) Handbook of clinical psychology. McGraw-Hill, New York, USA, pp 95–121Google Scholar
  11. Distel H, Ayabe-Kanamura S, Martínez-Gómez M, Schicker I, Kobayakawa T, Saito S, Hudson R (1999) Perception of everyday odors - correlation between intensity, familiarity and strength of hedonic judgment. Chem Senses 24(2):191–199CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dorries KM, Schmidt HJ, Beauchamp GK, Wysocki CJ (1989) Changes in sensitivity to the odor of androstenone during adolescence. Dev Psychobiol 22(5):423–435CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Doty RL, Laing DG (2003) Psychophysical measurement of human olfactory function, including odorant mixture assessment. In: Doty RL (ed) Handbook of olfaction and gustation, 2nd edn. Marcel Dekker, New York, USA, pp 203–228Google Scholar
  14. Eggert DD (1974) Eysenck-Persönlichkeits-Inventar. Hogrefe, Goettingen, GermanyGoogle Scholar
  15. Gower DB, Ruparelia BA (1993) Olfaction in humans with special reference to odorous 16-androstenes: their occurrence, perception and possible social, psychological and sexual impact. J Endocrinol 137(2):167–187CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Gower DB, Bird S, Sharman P, House FR (1985) Axillary 4-alpha-androst-16-en-3-one in men and women: relationships with olfactory acuity to odorous 16-androstenes. Experentia 41(9):1134–1136CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Hummel T, Gollisch R, Wildt G, Kobal G (1991) Changes in olfactory perception during the menstrual cycle. Experentia 47(7):712–715CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hummel T, Krone F, Lundström JN, Bartsch O (2005) Androstadienone odor thresholds in adolescents. Horm Behav 47(3):306–310CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Jacob TJC, Wang L, Jaffer S, McPhee S (2006) Changes in the odor quality of androstadienone during exposure-induced sensitization. Chem Senses 31(1):3–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Keller A, Zhuang H, Chi Q, Vosshall LB, Matsunami H (2007) Genetic variation in a human odorant receptor alters odour perception. Nature 449(7161):468–472CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Kirk-Smith MD, Booth DA (1980) Effects of androstenone on choice of location in others' presence. In: van der Starre H (ed) Olfaction and taste, vol. 7. IRL Press, London, UK, pp 389–392Google Scholar
  22. Knaapila A, Tuorila H, Silventoinen K, Wright MJ, Kyvik KO, Cherkas LF, Keskitalo K, Hansen J, Martin NG, Spector TD, Kaprio J, Perola M (2008) Genetic and environmental contributions to perceived intensity and pleasantness of androstenone odor: an international twin study. Chemosens Percept 1(1):34–42CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kranz F, Ishai A (2006) Face perception is modulated by sexual preference. Curr Biol 16(1):63–68CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. LeDoux J (2002) Synaptic self. How our brains become who we are. Macmillan, London, UKGoogle Scholar
  25. Loraine JA, Adamopoulos DA, Kirkham KE, Ismail AAA, Dove GA (1971) Patterns of hormone excretion in male and female homosexuals. Nature 234(5331):552–555CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Lundström JN, Hummel T, Olsson MJ (2003) Individual differences in sensitivity to the odor of 4, 16-androstadien-3-one. Chem Senses 28(7):643–650CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Lundström JN, Boyle JA, Zatorre RJ, Jones-Gotman M (2008) Functional neuronal processing of body odors differs from that of similar common odors. Cereb Cortex 18(6):1466–1474CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Martins Y, Preti G, Crabtree CR, Runyan T, Vainius AA, Wysocki CJ (2005) Preference for human body odor is influenced by gender and sexual orientation. Psychol Sci 16(9):694–701CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Menashe I, Abaffy T, Hasin Y, Goshe S, Yahalom V, Luetje CW, Lancet D (2007) Genetic elucidation of human hyperosmia to isovaleric acid. PLOS Biol 5(11):2462–2468CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Neave N, Menaged M, Weightman DR (1999) Sex differences in cognition: the role of testosterone and sexual orientation. Brain Cogn 41(3):245–262CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Nixon A, Mallet AI, Gower DB (1988) Simultaneous quantification of five odorous steroids (16-androstenes) in the axillary hair of men. J Steroid Biochem 29(5):505–510CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Pause B (2004) Are androgen steroids acting as pheromones in humans? Physiol Behav 83(1):21–29Google Scholar
  33. Pause BM, Rogalski KP, Sojka B, Ferstl R (1999) Sensitivity to androstenone in female subjects is associated with an altered brain response to male body odor. Physiol Behav 68(1–2):129–137CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Pause BM, Krauel K, Schrader C, Sojka B, Westphal E, Müller-Ruchholtz W, Ferstl R (2006) The human brain is a detector of chemosensorily transmitted HLA-class 1-similarity in same- and opposite-sex relations. Proc R Soc Lond B 273(1585):471–478CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Preti G, Cutler WB, Christensen CM, Lawley H, Huggins GR, Celso-Ramon G (1987) Human axillary extracts: analysis of compounds from samples which influence menstrual timing. J Chem Ecol 13(4):717–731CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Raja S, Stokes JP (1998) Assessing attitudes towards lesbians and gay men: the modern homophobia scale. J Gay Lesbian Bisex Identity 3(2):113–134Google Scholar
  37. Savic I, Berglund H, Gulyás B, Roland PE (2001) Smelling of odorous sex hormone-like compounds causes sex-differentiated hypothalamic activations in humans. Neuron 31(4):661–668CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Savic I, Berglund H, Lindström P (2005) Brain response to putative pheromones in homosexual men. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102(20):7356–7361CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Schmidt HJ, Beauchamp GK (1988) Adult-like odor preferences and aversions in three-year-old children. Child Dev 59(4):1136–1143CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Schneider-Düker M, Kohler A (1988) Die Erfassung von Geschlechtsrollen. Ergebnisse zur Deutschen Neukonstruktion des Bem-Sex-Role-Inventory. Diagnostica 34(3):256–270Google Scholar
  41. Stern K, McClintock M (1998) Regulation of ovulation by human pheromones. Nature 392(6672):177–179CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Welch BL (1947) The generalization of "student's" problem when several different population variances are involved. Biometrika 34(1–2):28–35Google Scholar
  43. Wyatt TD (2003) Pheromones and animal behaviour. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UKGoogle Scholar
  44. Wysocki CJ, Beauchamp GK (1984) Ability to smell androstenone is genetically determined. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 81(15):4899–4902CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Wysocki CJ, Beauchamp GK (1991) Individual differences in human olfaction. In: Wysocki K (ed) Chemical senses. Marcel Dekker, New York, USA, pp 353–373Google Scholar
  46. Wysocki CJ, Dorries KM, Beauchamp GK (1989) Ability to perceive androstenone can be acquired by ostensibly anosmic people. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 86(20):7976–7978CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Zeng X-N, Leyden JJ, Lawley HJ, Sawano K, Nohara I, Preti G (1991) Analysis of characteristic odors from human male axillae. J Chem Ecol 17(7):1469–1492CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Zucker KJ, Mitchell JN, Bradley SJ, Tkachuk J, Cantor JM, Allin SM (2006) The recalled childhood gender identity/gender role questionnaire: psychometric properties. Sex Roles 54(7–8):469–483CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  • Katrin Lübke
    • 1
  • Sylvia Schablitzky
    • 1
  • Bettina M. Pause
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Experimental PsychologyUniversity of DuesseldorfDuesseldorfGermany

Personalised recommendations