Advertisement

Annals of Dyslexia

, Volume 68, Issue 1, pp 1–14 | Cite as

Implicit sequence learning is preserved in dyslexic children

  • Filomena Inácio
  • Luís Faísca
  • Christian Forkstam
  • Susana Araújo
  • Inês Bramão
  • Alexandra Reis
  • Karl Magnus Petersson
Article
  • 282 Downloads

Abstract

This study investigates the implicit sequence learning abilities of dyslexic children using an artificial grammar learning task with an extended exposure period. Twenty children with developmental dyslexia participated in the study and were matched with two control groups—one matched for age and other for reading skills. During 3 days, all participants performed an acquisition task, where they were exposed to colored geometrical forms sequences with an underlying grammatical structure. On the last day, after the acquisition task, participants were tested in a grammaticality classification task. Implicit sequence learning was present in dyslexic children, as well as in both control groups, and no differences between groups were observed. These results suggest that implicit learning deficits per se cannot explain the characteristic reading difficulties of the dyslexics.

Keywords

Artificial grammar learning Children Dyslexia Implicit learning Reading acquisition 

Notes

Funding information

This work was supported by national Portuguese funding through Fundação para a Ciência e a Tecnologia, project ref. UID/BIM/04773/2013 CBMR, PTDC/MHC-PCN/1175/2014, and SFRH/BD/85439/2012.

References

  1. Blomert, L., & Vaessen, A. (2009). Differentiaal Diagnostiek van Dyslexie: cognitieve analyse van lezen en spellen [Dyslexia Differential Diagnosis: cognitive analysis of reading and spelling]. Amsterdam: Boom.Google Scholar
  2. Boets, B., Beeck, H. O., Vandermosten, M., Scott, S. K., Gillebert, C. R., Mantini, D., et al. (2013). Intact but less accessible phonetic representations in adults with dyslexia. Science, 342(6163), 1251–1254.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1244333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Cleeremans, A., Destrebecqz, A., & Boyer, M. (1998). Implicit learning: news from the front. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(10), 406–416.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01232-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Conway, C. M., Bauernschmidt, A., Huang, S. S., & Pisoni, D. B. (2010). Implicit statistical learning in language processing: word predictability is the key. Cognition, 114(3), 356–371.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.10.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Conway, C. M., & Pisoni, D. B. (2008). Neurocognitive basis of implicit learning of sequential structure and its relation to language processing. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1145, 113–131.  https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1416.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Ehri, L. C. (2005). Learning to read words: theory, findings, and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9(2), 167–188.  https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0902_4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Folia, V., Uddén, J., Forkstam, C., Ingvar, M., Hagoort, P., & Petersson, K. M. (2008). Implicit learning and dyslexia. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1145, 132–150.  https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1416.012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Franceschini, S., Gori, S., Ruffino, M., Pedrolli, K., & Facoetti, A. (2012). A causal link between visual spatial attention and reading acquisition. Current Biology, 22(9), 814–819.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.03.013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 177–190.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Hedenius, M., Persson, J., Alm, P. A., Ullman, M. T., Howard, J. H., Howard, D. V, & Jennische, M. (2013). Impaired implicit sequence learning in children with developmental dyslexia. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(11), 3924–3935.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.08.014
  11. Howard, J. H., Howard, D. V., Japikse, K. C., & Eden, G. F. (2006). Dyslexics are impaired on implicit higher-order sequence learning, but not on implicit spatial context learning. Neuropsychologia, 44(7), 1131–1144.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.10.015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Jiménez-Fernández, G., Vaquero, J. M. M., Jiménez, L., & Defior, S. (2010). Dyslexic children show deficits in implicit sequence learning, but not in explicit sequence learning or contextual cueing. Annals of Dyslexia, 61(1), 85–110.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-010-0048-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Kahta, S., & Schiff, R. (2016). Implicit learning deficits among adults with developmental dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 66(2), 235–250.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-016-0121-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Kaufman, S. B., Deyoung, C. G., Gray, J. R., Jiménez, L., Brown, J., & Mackintosh, N. (2010). Implicit learning as an ability. Cognition, 116(3), 321–340.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.05.011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Kelly, S. W., Griffiths, S., & Frith, U. (2002). Evidence for implicit sequence learning in dyslexia. Dyslexia, 8(1), 43–52.  https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Knowlton, B. J., & Squire, L. R. (1996). Artificial grammar learning depends on implicit acquisition of both abstract and exemplar-specific information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(1), 169–181.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.22.1.169.Google Scholar
  17. Laasonen, M., Vare, J., Oksanen-Hennah, H., Leppamaki, S., Tani, P., Harno, H., et al. (2014). Project DyAdd: implicit learning in adult dyslexia and ADHD. Annals of Dyslexia, 64(1), 1–33.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-013-0083-y.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Lum, J. A. G., Ullman, M. T., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2013). Procedural learning is impaired in dyslexia: evidence from a meta-analysis of serial reaction time studies. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(10), 3460–3476.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.07.017.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Lyon, G. R., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2003). Defining dyslexia, comorbidity, teachers’ knowledge of language and reading: a definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 53, 1–14.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-003-0001-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Menghini, D., Hagberg, G. E., Caltagirone, C., Petrosini, L., & Vicari, S. (2006). Implicit learning deficits in dyslexic adults: an fMRI study. NeuroImage, 33(4), 1218–1226.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.024.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Meulemans, T., & Van Der Linden, M. (1997). Associative chunk strength in artificial grammar learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23(4), 1007–1028.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.23.4.1007.Google Scholar
  22. Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (1999). Developmental dyslexia: the role of the cerebellum. Dyslexia, 5(3), 155–177.  https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0909.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J., & Dean, P. (2001). Developmental dyslexia: the cerebellar deficit hypothesis. Trends in Neurosciences, 24(9), 508–511  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01896-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Nieuwenhuis, I. L. C., Folia, V., Forkstam, C., Jensen, O., & Petersson, K. M. (2013). Sleep promotes the extraction of grammatical rules. PLoS One, 8(6), e65046.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0065046.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Nigro, L., Jiménez-Fernández, G., Simpson, I. C., & Defior, S. (2016). Implicit learning of non-linguistic and linguistic regularities in children with dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 66(2), 202–218.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-015-0116-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Pacheco, A., Reis, A., Araújo, S., Inácio, F., Petersson, K. M., & Faísca, L. (2014). Dyslexia heterogeneity: cognitive profiling of Portuguese children with dyslexia. Reading and Writing, 27(9), 1529–1545.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-014-9504-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Pavlidou, E. V., Kelly, M. L., & Williams, J. M. (2010). Do children with developmental dyslexia have impairments in implicit learning? Dyslexia, 16, 143–161.  https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Pavlidou, E. V., Williams, J. M., & Kelly, L. M. (2009). Artificial grammar learning in primary school children with and without developmental dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 59(1), 55–77.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-009-0023-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Pothos, E. M., & Kirk, J. (2004). Investigating learning deficits associated with dyslexia. Dyslexia, 10(1), 61–76.  https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Ramus, F., Rosen, S., Dakin, S. C., Day, B. L., Castellote, J. M., White, S., & Frith, U. (2003). Theories of developmental dyslexia: insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain, 126(4), 841–865.  https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awg076.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (2009). CPM-P Matrizes Progressivas Coloridas (Forma Paralela). Lisboa: Cegoc.Google Scholar
  32. Reber, A. S. (1967). Implicit learning of artificial grammars. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 855–863.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(67)80149-X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Roodenrys, S., & Dunn, N. (2008). Unimpaired implicit learning in children with developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia, 14, 1–15.  https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Rüsseler, J., Gerth, I., & Münte, T. F. (2006). Implicit learning is intact in adult developmental dyslexic readers: evidence from the serial reaction time task and artificial grammar learning. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28(5), 808–827.  https://doi.org/10.1080/13803390591001007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Santos, A. S., & Castro, S. L. (2010). Aprender a ler e avaliar a leitura. O TIL: teste de Idade de Leitura (2nd ed.). Coimbra: Almedina.Google Scholar
  36. Schmalz, X., Altoè, G., & Mulatti, C. (2017). Statistical learning and dyslexia: a systematic review. Annals of Dyslexia, 67(2), 147–162.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-016-0136-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Sela, I. (2014). Visual and auditory synchronization deficits among dyslexic readers as compared to non-impaired readers: a cross-correlation algorithm analysis. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 364.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Shaywitz, S. (2003). Overcoming dyslexia. New York: Vintage Books.Google Scholar
  39. Siegelman, N., Bogaerts, L., Christiansen, M. H., & Frost, R. (2017a). Towards a theory of individual differences in statistical learning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 372(1711), 20160059.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0059.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Siegelman, N., Bogaerts, L., & Frost, R. (2017b). Measuring individual differences in statistical learning: current pitfalls and possible solutions. Behavior Research Methods, 49(2), 418–432.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-016-0719-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Sperling, A. J., Lu, Z.-L., & Manis, F. R. (2004). Slower implicit categorical learning in adult poor readers. Annals of Dyslexia, 54(2), 281–303.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-004-0014-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Stoodley, C. J., Harrison, E. P. D., & Stein, J. F. (2006). Implicit motor learning deficits in dyslexic adults. Neuropsychologia, 44(5), 795–798.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.07.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Stoodley, C. J., & Stein, J. F. (2011). The cerebellum and dyslexia. Cortex, 47(1), 101–116.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2009.10.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Tijms, J. (2004). Verbal memory and phonological processing in dyslexia. Journal of Research in Reading, 27(3), 300–310.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2004.00233.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Trecy, M. P., Steve, M., & Martine, P. (2013). Impaired short-term memory for order in adults with dyslexia. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(7), 2211–2223.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.04.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Tunmer, W., & Greaney, K. (2011). Defining dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43(3), 229–243.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219409345009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. van Witteloostuijn, M., Boersma, P., Wijnen, F., & Rispens, J. (2017). Visual artificial grammar learning in dyslexia: a meta-analysis. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 70, 126–137.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2017.09.006
  48. Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): what have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(1), 2–40.  https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0021-9630.2003.00305.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Vicari, S., Marotta, L., Menghini, D., Molinari, M., & Petrosini, L. (2003). Implicit learning deficit in children with developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 41(1), 108–114.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00082-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Waber, D. P., Marcus, D. J., Forbes, P. W., Bellinger, D. C., Weiler, M. D., Sorensen, L. G., & Curran, T. (2003). Motor sequence learning and reading ability: is poor reading associated with sequencing deficits? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 84(4), 338–354.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-0965(03)00030-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: new evidence of bidirectional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30(1), 73–87.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.30.1.73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Wang, S., & Gathercole, S. E. (2013). Working memory deficits in children with reading difficulties: memory span and dual task coordination. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115(1), 188–197.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.11.015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Wechsler, D. (2006). Wechsler intelligence scale for children (3rd ed.). Lisboa: Cegoc.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© The International Dyslexia Association 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Cognitive Neuroscience Research Group, Center for Biomedical Research (CBMR)University of AlgarveFaroPortugal
  2. 2.Faculdade de Psicologia and Center for Psychological ResearchUniversidade de LisboaLisbonPortugal
  3. 3.Department of PsychologyLund UniversityLundSweden
  4. 4.Max Planck Institute for PsycholinguisticsNijmegenThe Netherlands

Personalised recommendations