Skip to main content
Log in

Elegant grapheme-phoneme correspondence: a periodic chart and singularity generalization unify decoding

  • Published:
Annals of Dyslexia Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The accompanying article introduces highly transparent grapheme-phoneme relationships embodied within a Periodic table of decoding cells, which arguably presents the quintessential transparent decoding elements. The study then folds these cells into one highly transparent but simply stated singularity generalization—this generalization unifies the decoding cells (97% transparency). Deeper, the periodic table and singularity generalization together highlight the connectivity of the periodic cells. Moreover, these interrelated cells, coupled with the singularity generalization, clarify teaching targets and enable efficient learning of the letter-sound code. This singularity generalization, in turn, serves as a model for creating unified but easily stated subordinate generalizations for any one of the transparent cells or groups of cells shown within the tables. The article then expands the periodic cells into two tables of teacher-ready sample word lists—one table includes sample words for the basic and phonogram vowel cells, and the other table embraces word samples for the transparent consonant cells. The paper concludes with suggestions for teaching the cellular transparency embedded within reoccurring isolated words and running text to promote decoding automaticity of the periodic cells.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allington, R. L., & McGill-Franzen, A. (Eds.). (2013). Summer reading: closing the rich/poor reading achievement gap. New York: Teachers College Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnesen, A., Braeken, J., Baker, S., Meek-Hansen, W., Ogden, T., & Melby-Lervag, M. (2016). Growth of oral reading fluency in a semitransparent orthography: concurrent and predictive relations with reading proficiency in Norwegian, grades 2-5. Reading Research Quarterly, 52(2), 177–201. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baker, E. A. (2017). Apps, iPads, and literacy: examining the feasibility of speech recognition in a first-grade classroom. Reading Research Quarterly, 52(3), 291–310. https://doi.org/10.1002/RRQ.170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Betts, E. A. (1955). What about phonics? Education, 75(May), 547–559.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, E. M. (1930). The value of phonics. New York State Education, 18(November), 266–267.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clymer, T. (1996/1963). The utility of phonic generalizations in the primary grades. The Reading Teacher, 50(3), 182–187. (Original work published 1963).

  • Duke, N. K. (2014, April 16). Limitations of broad phonics generalizations: when two vowels go walking, the first one doesn’t necessarily do the talking! http://www.literacyworldwide.org/blog/literacy-daily/2014/04/16/limitations-of-broad-phonics-generalizations-when-two-vowels-go-walking-the-first-one-doesn-t-necessarily-do-the-talking.

  • Gates, A. I. (1928). New methods in primary reading. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hanna, P. R., Hanna, J. S., Hodges, R. E., & Rudorf, E. H. (1966). Phoneme-grapheme correspondence as clues to spelling improvement. Washington, DC: United States Department of Health, Education, and Welfare.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hintikka, S., Landerl, K., Aro, M., & Lyytinen, H. (2008). Training reading fluency: is it important to practice aloud and is generalization possible? Annals of Dyslexia, 58(1), 59–79. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-008-0012-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, F. P. (2001). The utility of phonics generalizations: let’s take another look at Clymer’s conclusions. The Reading Teacher, 55(2), 132–143.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kress, E., & Fry, E. B. (2015). The reading teacher’s book of lists (6th ed.). Hoboken: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lightman, A. (2013). The accidental universe: the world you thought you knew. New York: Vintage Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meese, R. L. (2016). We’re not in Kansas anymore: the TOTO strategy for decoding vowel pairs. The Reading Teacher, 69(5), 549–552. https://doi.org/10.1002/trtr.1393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paige, D. D., Rasinski, T. V., & Magpuri-Lavell, T. (2012). Is fluent, expressive reading important for high school readers? Journal of Adolescent & Adult Literacy, 56(1), 67–76. https://doi.org/10.1002/JAAL.00103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pei, M. (1966). Glossary of linguistic terminology. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Randall, L. (2015). Dark matter and the universe: the astounding interconnectedness of the universe. New York: HarperCollins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rasinski, T. (2017). Readers who struggle: why many struggle and a modest proposal for improving their reading. The Reading Teacher, 70(5), 519–524. https://doi.org/10.1002/trtr.1533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sayeski, K. L., Earle, G. A., Eslinger, R. P., & Whitenton, J. N. (2016). Teacher candidates’ mastery of phoneme-grapheme correspondence: massed versus distributed practice in teacher education. Annals of Dyslexia, 67(1), 26–41. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-016-0126-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. (2017). Language at the speed of sight: how we read, why so many can’t, and what can be done about it. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stahl, S., Duffy-Hester, A., & Stahl, K. (1998). Everything you wanted to know about phonics (but were afraid to ask). Reading Research Quarterly, 33(3), 338–355. https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.33.3.5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Gorp, K., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2017a). Enhancing decoding efficiency in poor readers via a word identification game. Reading Research Quarterly, 52(1), 105–123. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Gorp, k., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2017b). The role of feedback and differences between good and poor decoders in a repeated word reading paradigm in first grade. Annals of Dyslexia, 67 (1), 1-25.

  • Willingham, D. T. (2017). The reading mind: a cognitive approach to understanding how the mind reads. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willis, J. (2006). Research-based strategies to ignite student learning. Alexandria, Virginia: ASCD.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yoncheva, Y., Wise, J., & McCandliss, B. (2015). Hemispheric specialization for visual words is shaped by attention to sublexical units during initial learning. Brain and Language, 145-146(June–July), 23–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2015.04.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeno, S. M., Ivens, S. H., Millard, R. T., & Duvvuri, R. (1995). The educator’s word frequency guide. Brewster: Touchtone Applied Science Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: a psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131(3), 3–29. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Louis Gates.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gates, L. Elegant grapheme-phoneme correspondence: a periodic chart and singularity generalization unify decoding. Ann. of Dyslexia 68, 69–83 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-017-0150-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-017-0150-x

Keywords

Navigation