Advertisement

Annals of Dyslexia

, Volume 67, Issue 1, pp 63–84 | Cite as

Phonemic—Morphemic dissociation in university students with dyslexia: an index of reading compensation?

  • Eddy Cavalli
  • Lynne G. Duncan
  • Carsten Elbro
  • Abdessadek El Ahmadi
  • Pascale Colé
Article

Abstract

A phonological deficit constitutes a primary cause of developmental dyslexia, which persists into adulthood and can explain some aspects of their reading impairment. Nevertheless, some dyslexic adults successfully manage to study at university level, although very little is currently known about how they achieve this. The present study investigated at both the individual and group levels, whether the development of another oral language skill, namely, morphological knowledge, can be preserved and dissociated from the development of phonological knowledge. Reading, phonological, and morphological abilities were measured in 20 dyslexic and 20 non-dyslexic university students. The results confirmed the persistence of deficits in phonological but not morphological abilities, thereby revealing a dissociation in the development of these two skills. Moreover, the magnitude of the dissociation correlated with reading level. The outcome supports the claim that university students with dyslexia may compensate for phonological weaknesses by drawing on morphological knowledge in reading.

Keywords

Dissociation Morpheme awareness Phoneme awareness University students with dyslexia 

Notes

Acknowledgments

The authors gratefully acknowledge Jennifer Martin for having allowed us to use her materials and Mélody Zira for her help in testing participants. In addition, we would like to thank all the participants in this study. Special thanks are due to Professor John R. Crawford for giving us permission to use and disseminate his single-case study method. Finally, we would also like to thank Florence Poracchia-George (CERTA, Salvator Hospital Marseille, France) for helping with the recruitment of dyslexic participants.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Society (2013) ‘Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5).’ <http://www.dsm5.org/Pages/Default.aspx> (accessed 10 July 2014).
  2. Anglin, J. (1993). Vocabulary development: a morphological analysis. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58(10, Serial No. 238).Google Scholar
  3. Arnbak, E., & Elbro, C. (2000). The effects of morphological awareness training on the reading and spelling skills of young dyslexics. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 44, 229–251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Boets, B., Op de Beeck, H. P., Vandermosten, M., Scott, S. K., Gillebert, C. R., Mantini, D., Bulthé, J., Sunaert, S., Wouters, J., & Ghesquière, P. (2013). Intact but less accessible phonetic representations in adults with dyslexia. Science, 342(6163), 1251–1254.Google Scholar
  5. Bogacz, R., Wagenmakers, E. J., Forstmann, B. U., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2010). The neural basis of the speed-accuracy tradeoff. Trends Neuroscience, 33, 10–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bruck, M. (1990). Word-recognition skills of adults with childhood diagnoses of dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 26, 439–454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bruck, M. (1992). Persistence of dyslexics’ phonological awareness deficits. Developmental Psychology, 28, 874–886.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Burani, C., Marcolini, S., De Luca, M., & Zoccolotti, P. (2008). Morpheme-based reading aloud: evidence from dyslexic and skilled Italian readers. Cognition, 108, 243–262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Callens, M., Tops, W., & Brysbaert, M. (2012). Cognitive profile of students who enter higher education with an indication of dyslexia. PloS One, 7, e38081.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Callens, M., Tops, W., Stevens, M., & Brysbaert, M. (2014). An exploratory factor analysis of the cognitive functioning of first-year bachelor students with dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 64, 91–119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Carlisle, J. F. (1987). The use of morphological knowledge in spelling derived forms by learning-disabled and normal students. Annals of dyslexia., 37, 90–108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Carlisle, J. F. (1995). Morphological awareness and early reading achievement. In L. B. Feldman (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 189–209). Hillsdale: NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  13. Casalis, S., Mathiot, E., Bécavin, A. S., & Colé, P. (2003). Conscience morphologique chez des lecteurs tout venant et en difficultés. Silexicales, 3, 57–66.Google Scholar
  14. Casalis, S., Colé, P., & Sopo, D. (2004). Morphological awareness in developmental dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 54, 114–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Cavalli, E., Casalis, S., El-Ahmadi, A., Zira, M., Poracchia-George, F., & Colé, P. (2016). Vocabulary skills are well developed in university students with dyslexia: evidence from multiple case studies. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 51-52, 89–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Colé, P., Casalis, S., Dufayard, C. (2012). Logiciel Morphorem. Isbergues, Ortho-Editions.Google Scholar
  17. Corkett, J. K., & Parrila, R. (2008). Use of context in the word recognition process by adults with a significant history of reading difficulties. Annals of Dyslexia, 58, 139–161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Crawford, J. R., & Garthwaite, P. H. (2005a). Evaluation of criteria for classical dissociations in single-case studies by Monte Carlo simulation. Neuropsychology, 19, 664–678.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Crawford, J. R., & Garthwaite, P. H. (2005b). Testing for suspected impairments and dissociations in single-case studies in neuropsychology: evaluation of alternatives using Monte Carlo simulations and revised tests for dissociations. Neuropsychology, 19, 318–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Crawford, J. R., & Garthwaite, P. H. (2007). Comparison of a single case to a control or normative sample in neuropsychology: development of a Bayesian approach. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24, 343–372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Crawford, J. R., Garthwaite, P. H., & Porter, S. (2010). Point and interval estimates of effect sizes for the case-controls design in neuropsychology: rationale, methods, implementations, and proposed reporting standards. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 27, 245–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Deacon, S. H., & Kirby, J. R. (2004). Morphological awareness: just “more phonological”? The roles of morphological and phonological awareness in reading development. Applied PsychoLinguistics, 25, 223–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Deacon, S. H., Parrila, R., & Kirby, J. R. (2006). Processing of derived forms in high-functioning dyslexics. Annals of Dyslexia, 56, 103–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Deacon, S. H., Tong, X., & Mimeau, C. (in press). Morphological and semantic processing in developmental dyslexia across languages: a theoretical and empirical review. In C. Perfetti & L. Verhoeven (Eds.), Dyslexia across languages and writing systems: a handbook. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  25. Di Betta, A. M., & Romani, C. (2006). Lexical learning and dysgraphia in a group of adults with developmental dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23, 376–400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Duncan, L. G., Casalis, S., & Colé, P. (2009). Early metalinguistic awareness of derivational morphology: observations from a comparison of English and French. Applied PsychoLinguistics, 30, 405–440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised.Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
  28. Dunn, L. M., Theriault-Whalen, C. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1993). Echelle de vocabulaire en images Peabody, adaptation française. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Psycan.Google Scholar
  29. Elbro, C. (1989). Morphological awareness in dyslexia. In C. Von Euler, I. Lundberg, & G. Lennerstrand (Eds.), Brain and reading: structural and functional anomalies in developmental dyslexia with special reference to interactions, memory functions, linguistic processes and visual analysis in reading (pp. 189–209). London: MacMillan.Google Scholar
  30. Elbro, C., & Arnbak, E. (1996). The role of morpheme recognition and morphological awareness in dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 46, 209–240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Elbro, C. (1986). Morphemes in the reading process. In K. Trondhjem (Ed.), Aspects in reading processes—with special regards to the education of hearing-impaired students. Twelvth Danavox Symposium (pp. 167–185). Copenhagen: Danavox.Google Scholar
  32. Elbro, C., Nielsen, I., & Petersen, D. K. (1994). Dyslexia in adults: evidence for deficits in non-word reading and in the phonological representation of lexical items. Annals of Dyslexia, 44, 205–226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Elbro, C., King, M., Rown, D., & Oakhill, J. (2016). How morphological knowledge can affect the process of word decoding. Porto, Portugal: Paper presented at the Twenty-Third Annual Meeting of the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading.Google Scholar
  34. Fowler, A. E., & Liberman, I. Y. (1995). The role of phonology and orthography in morphological awareness. In L. B. Feldman (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  35. Gombert, J. E. (1990). Le développement métalinguistique. Paris: PUF.Google Scholar
  36. Goodwin, A. P., & Ahn, S. (2010). A meta-analysis of morphological interventions: effects on literacy achievement of children with literacy difficulties. Annals of Dyslexia, 60, 183–208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Goswami, U., & Bryant, P. (1989). The interpretation of studies using the reading level design. Journal of Reading Behavior, 21, 413–424.Google Scholar
  38. Guo, Y., Roehrig, A. D., & Williams, R. S. (2011). The relation of morphological awareness and syntactic awareness to adults’ reading comprehension: is vocabulary knowledge a mediating variable? Journal of Literacy Research: A Publication of the Literacy Research Association, 43, 159–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Haft, S. L., Myers, C. A., & Hoeft, F. (2016). Socio-emotional and cognitive resilience in children with reading disabilities. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 10, 133–141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Helenius, P., Salmelin, R., Service, E., & Connolly, J. F. (1999). Semantic cortical activation in dyslexic readers. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11, 535–550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Kunert, R., & Scheepers, C. (2014). Speed and accuracy of dyslexic versus typical word recognition: an eye-movement investigation. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Law, J. M., Wouters, J., & Ghesquière, P. (2015). Morphological awareness and its role in compensation in adults with dyslexia. Dyslexia, 21, 254–272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Lefavrais, P. (2005). Alouette-R. Paris: ECPA.Google Scholar
  44. Leikin, M., & Zur Hagit, E. (2006). Morphological processing in adult dyslexia. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 35, 471–490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Leong, C. K. (1989). Productive knowledge of derivational rules in poor readers. Annals of Dyslexia, 39, 94–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Leong, C. K. (1999). Phonological and morphological processing in adult students with learn- ing/reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 224–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Litt, R. A., & Nation, K. (2014). The nature and specificity of paired associate learning deficits in children with dyslexia. Journal of Memory and Language, 71, 71–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Lyon, G. R., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2003). A definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 53, 1–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Martin, J., Colé, P., Leuwers, C., Casalis, S., Zorman, M., & Sprenger-Charolles, L. (2010). Reading in French-speaking adults with dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 60, 238–264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Martin, J., Frauenfelder, U. H., & Colé, P. (2013). Morphological awareness in dyslexic university students. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1–21.Google Scholar
  51. Nagy, W., Berninger, V. W., & Abott, R. D. (2006). Contribution of morphology beyond phonology to literacy outcomes of upper elementary and middle-school students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 134–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. New, B., Pallier, C., Ferrand, L., & Matos, R. (2001). Une base de données lexicales du français contemporain sur internet : LEXIQUE. L’année psychologique, 101, 447–462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Quémart, P., & Casalis, S. (2013). Visual processing of derivational morphology in children with developmental dyslexia: Insights from masked priming. Applied Psycholinguistics (pp. 1–32).Google Scholar
  54. Ramus, F., Rosen, S., Dakin, S. C., Day, B. L., Castellote, J. M., White, S., & Frith, U. (2003). Theories of developmental dyslexia: insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain, 126, 841–865.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1995). Raven manual: Coloured progressive matrices. Oxford, UK: Oxford Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
  56. Re, A. M., Tressoldi, P. E., Cornoldi, C., & Lucangeli, D. (2011). Which tasks best discriminate between dyslexic university students and controls in a transparent language? Dyslexia, 17, 227–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Reid, A. A., Szczerbinski, M., Iskierka-Kasperek, E., & Hansen, P. C. (2006). Cognitive profiles of adult developmental dyslexics: theoretical implications. Dyslexia, 13, 1–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Rey-Debove, J. (1984). Le domaine de la morphologie lexicale [The domain of lexical morphology. Cahiers de Lexicologie, 45, 3–19.Google Scholar
  59. Richlan, F., Kronbichler, M., & Wimmer, H. (2011). Meta-analyzing brain dysfunctions in dyslexic children and adults. NeuroImage, 56, 1735–1742.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Romani, C., Di Betta, A. M., Tsouknida, E., & Olson, A. (2008). Lexical and nonlexical processing in developmental dyslexia: a case for different resources and different impairments. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25, 798–830.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Rüsseler, J., Becker, P., Johannes, S., & Münte, T. F. (2007). Semantic, syntactic, and phonological processing of written words in adult developmental dyslexic readers: an event-related brain potential study. BMC Neuroscience, 8, 52.Google Scholar
  62. Schiff, R., & Raveh, M. (2007). Deficient morphological processing in adults with developmental dyslexia: another barrier to efficient word recognition? Dyslexia, 13, 110–129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Schiff, R., & Ravid, D. (2007). Morphological analogies in Hebrew-speaking university students with dyslexia compared with typically developing gradeschoolers. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 36, 237–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2005). Dyslexia (specific reading disability. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1301–1309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Pugh, K. R., Fulbright, R. K., Mencl, W. E., Constable, R. T., Skudlarsi, P., Fletcher, J. M., Lyon, G. R., & Gore, J. C. (2001). The neurobiology of dyslexia. Clinical Neuroscience Research, 1, 291–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Shaywitz, S. E., Mody, M., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2006). Neural mechanisms in dyslexia. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 278–281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Sprenger-Charolles, L., & Colé, P. (2003). Lecture et dyslexie. Paris, Dunod.Google Scholar
  68. Sprenger-Charolles, L., Colé, P., Béchennec, D., & Kipffer-Piquard, A. (2005). French normative data on reading and related skills from EVALEC, a new computerized battery of tests (end grade 1, grade 2, grade 3, and grade 4). Revue Européenne de Psychologie Appliquée/European Review of Applied Psychology, 55, 157–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Suárez-Coalla, P., & Cuetos, F. (2015). Reading difficulties in Spanish adults with dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 65, 33–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Swanson, H. L. (2012). Adults with reading disabilities: converting a meta-analysis to practice. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 45, 17–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Swanson, H. L., & Hsieh, C.-J. (2009). Reading disabilities in adults: a selective meta-analysis of the literature. Review of Educational Research, 79, 1362–1390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Tops, W., Callens, M., Bijn, E., & Brysbaert, M. (2012). Spelling in adolescents with dyslexia: errors and modes of assessment. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 47, 295–306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Tractenberg, R. E. (2002). Exploring hypotheses about phonological awareness, memory, and reading achievement. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 407–424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Tsesmeli, S., & Seymour, P. (2006). Derivational morphology and spelling in dyslexia. Reading and Writing, 19, 587–625.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Vellutino, F. R., Scanlon, D. M., & Spearing, D. (1995). Semantic and phonological coding in poor and normal readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 59, 76–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© The International Dyslexia Association 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Aix-Marseille Université, LPC, CNRSMarseilleFrance
  2. 2.School of PsychologyUniversity of DundeeScotlandUK
  3. 3.Centre for Reading ResearchUniversity of CopenhagenCopenhagenDenmark
  4. 4.Aix-Marseille Université, LNIA, CNRSMarseilleFrance

Personalised recommendations