Abstract
Recent findings suggest that people with dyslexia experience difficulties with the learning of serial order information during the transition from short- to long-term memory (Szmalec et al. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition 37(5): 1270-1279, 2011). At the same time, models of short-term memory increasingly incorporate a distinction of order and item processing (Majerus et al. Cognition 107: 395-419, 2008). The current study is aimed to investigate whether serial order processing deficiencies in dyslexia can be traced back to a selective impairment of short-term memory for serial order and whether this impairment also affects processing beyond the verbal domain. A sample of 26 adults with dyslexia and a group of age and IQ-matched controls participated in a 2 × 2 × 2 experiment in which we assessed short-term recognition performance for order and item information, using both verbal and nonverbal material. Our findings indicate that, irrespective of the type of material, participants with dyslexia recalled the individual items with the same accuracy as the matched control group, whereas the ability to recognize the serial order in which those items were presented appeared to be affected in the dyslexia group. We conclude that dyslexia is characterized by a selective impairment of short-term memory for serial order, but not for item information, and discuss the integration of these findings into current theoretical views on dyslexia and its associated dysfunctions.
This is a preview of subscription content,
to check access.
Notes
To avoid confusion with the distinction between sensory modalities like the visual versus auditory modality, we will use the term domain here to dissociate processing of verbal from that of nonverbal material. Most of our stimulus material was presented visually, in which one experimental factor, labeled domain, varied verbal versus nonverbal content.
References
Beaton, A. A. (2004). Dyslexia, reading and the brain. A sourcebook of psychological and biological research. New York: Psychology Press.
Barca, L., Burani, C., Di Filippo, G., & Zoccolotti, P. (2006). Italian developmental dyslexics and proficient readers: Where are the differences? Brain and Language, 98, 347–351.
Bosse, M.-L., Tainturier, J. M., & Valdois, S. (2007). Developmental dyslexia: The visual attention span deficit hypothesis. Cognition, 104, 198–230.
Brosnan, M., Demetre, J., Hamill, S., Robson, K., Shepherd, H., & Cody, G. (2002). Executive functioning in adults and children with developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 40, 2144–2155.
Brown, G. D. A., Preece, T., & Hulme, C. (2000). Oscillator-based memory for serial order. Psychological Review, 107, 127–181.
Brus, B. T., & Voeten, M. J. M. (1979). Een-Minuut-Test. Vorm A en B. Schoolvorderingentest voor de technische leesvaardigheid, bestemd voor groep 4 tot en met 8 van het basisonderwijs. Verantwoording en Handleiding [One-Minute-Test. Forms A and B. Scholastic aptitude test for technical reading comprehension, for Group 4 up to 8 of primary school. Justification and manual]. Nijmegen, The Netherlands: Berkhout
Burgess, N., & Hitch, G. J. (1999). Memory for serial order: A network model of the phonological loop and its timing. Psychological Review, 106(3), 551–581.
Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (2004). Is there a causal link from phonological awareness to success in learning to read? Cognition, 91, 77–111.
Callens, M., Tops, W., & Brysbaert, M. (2012). Cognitive profile of students who enter higher education with an indication of dyslexia. PLoS One, 7(6), e38081. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0038081.
Costanzo, F., Menghini, D., Caltagirone, C., Oliveri, M., & Vicari, S. (2012). High frequency rTMS over the left parietal lobule increases non-word reading accuracy. Neuropsychologia, 50(11), 2645–2651.
Davis, R. D. (2010). The gift of dyslexia: Why some of the smartest people can’t read and how they can learn. London: Souvenir Press.
De Pessemier, P., & Andries, C. (2009). Gletschr. Test voor Gevorderd Lezen & Schrijven [Gletschr. Test for Advanced Reading & Writing]. Antwerpen: Garant.
Dekker, R., Dekker, P. H., & Mulder, J. L. (2004). Kaufman-Intelligentietest voor Adolescenten en Volwassenen, Handleiding [Kaufman Adolescent and Adult Intelligence Test, Manual]. Leiden, The Netherlands: PITS.
Facoetti, A., Trussardi, A. N., Ruffino, M., Larusso, M. L., Cattaneo, C., Galli, R., et al. (2009). Multisensory spatial attention deficits are predictive of phonological decoding skills in developmental dyslexia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(5), 1011–1025.
Farrell, S., & Lewandowsky, S. (2002). An endogenous distributed model of ordering in serial recall. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 59–79.
Friedmann, N., & Rahamim, E. (2007). Developmental letter position dyslexia. Journal of Neuropsychology, 1(2), 201–236.
Goswami, U. (2003). Why theories about developmental dyslexia require developmental designs. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7, 534–540.
Gupta, P., Lipinski, J., & Actunc, E. (2005). Reexamining the phonological similarity effect in immediate serial recall: The roles of type of similarity, category cuing, and item recall. Memory and Cognition, 33, 1001–1016.
Hebb, D. (1961). Distinctive features of learning in the higher animal. In J. F. Delafresnaye (Ed.), Brain mechanisms and learning (pp. 37–46). Oxford: Blackwell.
Hitch, G. J., Flude, B., & Burgess, N. (2009). Slave to the rhythm: Experimental tests of a model for verbal short-term memory and long-term sequence learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 61(1), 97–111.
Howard, J. H., Jr., Howard, D. V., Japikse, K. C., & Eden, G. F. (2006). Dyslexics are impaired on implicit higher-order sequence learning, but not on implicit spatial context learning. Neuropsychologia, 44, 1131–1144.
Katz, R. B., Shankweiler, D., & Liberman, I. Y. (1981). Memory for item order and phonetic recoding in the beginning reader. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 32(3), 474–484.
Kibby, M. Y., Marks, W., Morgan, S., & Long, C. J. (2004). Specific impairment in developmental reading disabilities: A working memory approach. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 37(4), 349–363.
Lallier, M., Donnadieu, S., Berger, C., & Valdois, S. (2010). A case study of developmental phonological dyslexia: Is the attentional deficit in the perception of rapid stimuli sequences amodal? Cortex, 46, 231–241.
Lobier, M., Zoubrinetzky, R., & Valdois, S. (2012). The visual attention span deficit in dyslexia is visual and not verbal. Cortex, 48, 768–773.
Luck, S. J., & Vogel, E. K. (1997). The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature, 390, 279–281.
Majerus, S., D’Argembeau, A., Martinez Perez, T., Belayachi, S., Van der Linden, M., Collette, F., et al. (2009). The commonality of neural networks for verbal and visual short-term memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(11), 2570–2593.
Majerus, S., Poncelet, P., Elsen, B., & van der Linden, M. (2006). Exploring the relationship between new word learning and short-term memory for serial order recall, item recall, and item recognition. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 18, 848–873.
Majerus, S., Poncelet, P., Van der Linden, M., & Weekes, B. S. (2008). Lexical learning in bilingual adults: The relative importance of short-term memory for serial order and phonological knowledge. Cognition, 107, 395–419.
Martinez Perez, T., Majerus, S., Mahot, A., & Poncelet, M. (2012). Evidence for a specific impairment of serial order short-term memory in dyslexic children. Dyslexia, 18(2), 94–109.
May, C. P., Hasher, L., & Kane, M. J. (1999). The role of interference in memory span. Memory and Cognition, 27(5), 759–767.
McCandliss, B. D., Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2003). The visual work form area: Expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7(7), 293–299.
Morais, J., Gary, L., Alegria, J., & Bertelson, P. (1979). Does awareness of speech as a sequence of phones arise spontaneously? Cognition, 7, 323–331.
Morais, J., & Kolinsky, R. (1994). Perception and awareness in phonological processing: The case of the phoneme. Cognition, 50(1–3), 287–297.
Mosse, E. K., & Jarrold, C. (2008). Hebb learning, verbal short-term memory, and the acquisition of phonological forms in children. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(4), 505–514.
Nairne, J., & Kelley, M. (2004). Separating item and order information through process dissociation. Journal of Memory and Language, 50(2), 113–133.
Nithart, C., Demont, E., Majerus, S., Leybaert, J., Poncelet, M., & Metz-Lutz, M. N. (2009). Reading disabilities in SLI and dyslexia result from distinct phonological impairments. Developmental Neuropsychology, 34, 296–311.
Page, M. P. A., & Norris, D. (2009). A model linking immediate serial recall, the Hebb repetition effect and the learning of phonological word forms. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1536), 3737–3753.
Pavlidou, E. V., Kelly, M., & Williams, J. M. (2010). Do children with developmental dyslexia have impairments in implicit learning? Dyslexia, 16(2), 143–161.
Pennington, B. F. (2006). From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders. Cognition, 101, 385–413.
Pennington, B. F., Bennetto, L., McAleer, O., & Roberts, R. J., Jr. (1996). Executive functions and working memory: Theoretical and measurement issues. In R. G. Lyon & N. A. Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention, memory, and executive function (pp. 327–348). Baltimore: Paul H Brookes.
Pennington, B. F., Van Orden, G., Kirson, D., & Haith, M. (1991). What is the causal relation between verbal STM problems and dyslexia? In S. A. Brady & D. P. Shankweiler (Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy. A tribute to Isabelle Y Liberman (173-186). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Ramus, F., & Szenkovits, G. (2008). What phonological deficit? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 129–141.
Romani, C., Tsouknika, E., di Betta, A. M., & Olson, A. (2011). Reduced attentional capacity, but normal processing speed and shifting of attention in developmental dyslexia: Evidence from a serial task. Cortex, 47, 715–733.
Roodenrys, S., & Dunn, N. (2008). Unimpaired implicit learning in children with developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia, 14, 1–15.
Rossion, B., & Pourtois, G. (2004). Revisiting Snodgrass and Vanderwart’s object set: The role of surface detail in basic-level object recognition. Perception, 33, 217–236.
Saint-Aubin, J., & Poirier, M. (1999). The influence of long-term memory factors on immediate serial recall: An item and order analysis. International Journal of Psychology, 34(5–6), 347–352.
Severens, E., van Lommel, S., Ratinckx, E., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2005). Timed picture naming norms for 590 pictures in Dutch. Acta Psychologica, 119(2), 159–187.
Smith-Spark, J. H., & Fisk, J. E. (2007). Working memory functioning in developmental dyslexia. Memory, 15(1), 34–56.
Swan, D., & Goswami, U. (1997). Picture naming deficit in developmental dyslexia: The phonological representations hypothesis. Brain and Language, 56, 334–353.
Szmalec, A., Duyck, W., Vandierendonck, A., Barbera Mata, A., & Page, M. P. A. (2009). The Hebb repetition effect as a laboratory analogue of novel word learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(3), 435–443.
Szmalec, A., Loncke, M., Page, M. P. A., & Duyck, W. (2011). Order or disorder? Impaired Hebb learning in dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 37(5), 1270–1279.
Szmalec, A., Page, M. P. A., & Duyck, W. (2012). The development of long-term lexical representations through Hebb repetition learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 67(3), 342–354.
Tops, W., Callens, M., Lammertyn, J., Van Hees, V., & Brysbaert, M. (2012). Identifying students with dyslexia in higher education. Annals of Dyslexia, 62(3), 186–203.
Van den Bos, K. P., Lutje Spelberg, H. C., Scheepsma, A. J. M., & de Vries, J. R. (1994). De Klepel. Vorm A en B. Een test voor de leesvaardigheid van pseudowoorden. Verantwoording Handleiding Diagnostiek en behandeling [The Klepel. Forms A and B. A test for reading ability of pseudowords. Justification manual diagnostics and treatment]. Nijmegen: Berkhout.
Van Selst, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (1994). A solution to the effect of sample size on outlier elimination. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 47(3), 631–650.
Vicari, S., Marotta, L., Menghini, D., Molinari, M., & Petrosini, L. (2003). Implicit learning deficit in children with developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 41, 108–114.
Vidyasagar, T. R., & Pammer, K. (2009). Dyslexia: A deficit in visuo-spatial attention, not in phonological processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(2), 57–63.
Waber, D. P., Marcus, D. J., Forbes, P. W., Bellinger, D. C., Weiler, M. D., Sorensen, L. G., et al. (2003). Motor sequence learning and reading ability: Is poor reading associated with sequencing deficits? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 84, 338–354.
Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3–29.
Zoccolotti, P., & Friedmann, N. (2010). From dyslexia to dyslexias, from dysgraphia to dysgraphias, from a cause to causes: A look at current research on developmental dyslexia and dysgraphia. Cortex, 46, Special issue: Editorial, 1211–1215.
Acknowledgments
The first author received a stipend to attend the doctoral school in Cognitive Science and Education at the Department of Psychology and Cognitive Science of the University of Trento, Italy. This research was further supported in part by a grant from the Flemish Agency for Persons with a Handicap (Vlaams Agentschap voor Personen met een Handicap). The funding sources were not involved in any research-related step of this work. We thank three anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on earlier versions of this article, and we are grateful to all participants for their dedication, to Valerie Van Hees and Charlotte De Langhe of Cursief for their invaluable support to find dyslexic volunteers, and to Maaike Callens, Wim Tops, and Marc Brysbaert for providing us with their data on reading tests and IQ for several participants.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
Highlights
• Adult dyslexics were tested on short-term memory (STM) tasks for order and item
• Both conditions were run with verbal and nonverbal material, respectively
• Dyslexics compared to controls performed worse in order tasks but not in item tasks
• Data suggest a domain-general problem with serial order in STM to underlie dyslexia
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Hachmann, W.M., Bogaerts, L., Szmalec, A. et al. Short-term memory for order but not for item information is impaired in developmental dyslexia. Ann. of Dyslexia 64, 121–136 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-013-0089-5
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-013-0089-5