Abstract
There have been relatively few single case studies concerned with the remediation of spelling deficits among developmental impairments. Among these there have been a small number that targeted specific components of the spelling process and used linguistic theories as theoretical underpinning for the development of remediation procedures. This single case study examines remediation of writing skills and aims at evaluating two different lexically based intervention methods, one of which used Optimality Theory as its basis. We applied a rule-based remediation and an intervention method using whole-word forms to a child with selective impairments in the lexical-graphemic components. The investigation was done with words in which phoneme-grapheme-correspondences in word final position change due to voicing neutralization. The individual exhibited a method- and item-specific effect with respect to the rule-based method. In addition, a transfer effect to untreated items and a generalization effect to untrained but related tasks was observed. The absence of a method-specific and a generalization effect for the whole-word form intervention and the success of the rule-based method is determined by the specific cognitive component(s)s that constitute the source of the deficit and the appropriateness of Optimality Theory to address this particular deficit.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.
References
Aliminosa, D., Mcloskey, M., Goodman-Schulman, R., & Sokol, S. (1993). Remediation of acquired dysgraphia as a technique for testing interpretation of deficits. Aphasiology, 7, 55–69.
Baddeley, A. (1993). A theory of rehabilitation without a model of learning is a vehicle without an engine: A comment on Caramazza and Hillis. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 3, 235–244.
Barlow, D. H., & Hersen, M. (1984). Single case experimental designs. Strategies for studying behavior change. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Beauvois, M.-F., & Derouesné, J. (1981). Lexical or orthographic agraphia. Brain, 104, 21–49.
Beeson, P. M. (1999). Treating acquired writing impairment: Strengthening graphemic representations. Aphasiology, 13, 367–386.
Beeson, P. M., Rewega, M., Vail, S., & Rapcsak, S. Z. (2000). Problem-solving approach to agraphia treatment: Interactive use of lexical and sublexical spelling routes. Aphasiology, 14, 551–565.
Behrmann, M. (1987). The rites of righting writing: Homophone remediation in acquired dysgraphia. Cognitve Neuropsychology, 4, 365–384.
Behrmann, M., & Byng, S. (1992). A cognitive approach to the neurorehabilitation of acquired language disorders. In D. I. Margolin (Ed.). Cognitive neuropsychology in clinical practice (pp. 327–350). New York: Oxford University Press.
Boersma, P. (1998). Functional phonology: Formalizing the interactions between articulation and perceptual drives. The Hague: Holland Academic Graphics.
Caramazza, A. (1986). On drawing inferences about the structure of normal cognitive systems from the analysis of patterns of impaired performance: The case for single-patient studies. Brain and Cognition, 5, 41–66.
Caramazza, A. (1988). Some aspects of language processing revealed through the analysis of acquired dysgraphia: The lexical system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 11, 395–421.
Caramazza, A., & Hillis, A. (1993). For a theory of remediation of cognitive deficits. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 3, 217–234.
Carlomagno, S., Iavarone, A., & Colombo, A. (1994). Cognitive approaches to writing rehabilitation: From single case to group studies. In M. J. Riddoch & G. W. Humphreys (Eds.). Cognitive neuropsychology and cognitive rehabilitation (pp. 485–502). Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Cattell, R. B. (1960). Cultural fair intelligence test, scale 2. Champaign, IL: IPAT.
Coltheart, M. (1983). Aphasia therapy research: A single case study approach. In C. Code & D. J. Muller (Eds.). Aphasia therapy (pp. 194–202). London: Edward Arnold.
De Partz, M.-P., Seron, X., & van der Linden, M. V. (1992). Re-education of surface dysgraphia with a visual imagery strategy. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 9, 369–401.
Ellis, A. (1989). Reading, writing and dyslexia. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Grijzenhout, J. (2001). West-Germanic sound structure: A contrastive study. Habilitationsschrift, Heinrich Heine Universität, Düsseldorf.
Hatfield, F., & Patterson, K. E. (1983). Phonological spelling. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35A, 451–468.
Hillis, A. E. (1989). Efficacy and generalization for aphasic naming errors. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 70, 632–636.
Kager, R. (1999). Optimality theory. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Leonard, L. B. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
McCarthy, J., & Prince, A. (1993). Prosodic morphology I: Constraint interaction and satisfaction. Manuscript, Amherst, MA: University of Massachusetts, and New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University.
McCarthy, J., & Prince, A. (1994). The emergence of the unmarked: Optimality in prosodic morphology. In M. González (Ed.). Papers of the 24th annual meeting of the north eastern linguistic society (pp. 333–379). Amherst, MA: GLSA.
Patterson, K. E. (1988). Acquired disorders of spelling. In G. Denes, C. Semenza, & P. Bissiachi (Eds.). Perspectives on cognitive neuropsychology. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Prince, A., & Smolensky, P. (1993). Optimality theory: Constraint interaction in generative grammar (Report RuCCS-TR-2). New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University, Center for Cognitive Science.
Riddoch, M. J., & Humphreys, G. W. (Eds.). (1994). Cognitive neuropsychology and cognitive rehabilitation. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Seron, X., & Deloche, G. (1989). Cognitive approaches in neuropsychological rehabilitation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to mental structure. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Siegel, S., & Castellan, N. J. (1988). Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Temple, C. (1985). Developmental surface dysgraphia: A case report. Applied Psycholinguistics, 6, 391–406.
Temple, C. (1986). Developmental surface dysgraphias. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38A(6), 391–406.
Temple, C. (1990). Foop is still floop: A six-year follow-up of phonological dyslexia and dysgraphia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 209–221.
Temple, C. (1997). Developmental cognitive neuropsychology. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Temple, C., & Carney, R. A. (1996). Reading skills in children with Turner’s syndrome: An analysis of hyperlexia. Cortex, 32, 335–345.
Weekes, B., & Coltheart, M. (1996). Surface dyslexia and surface dysgraphia: Treatment studies and their theoretical implications. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13, 277–315.
Weiss, R., & Osterland, J. (1997). Grundintelligenztest. Skala 1 [Basic intelligence test. Scale 1]. Bern: Hogrefe: Verlag für Psychologie.
Wiese, R. (1996). The phonology of German. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Wilson, B. (2002). Towards a comprehensive model of cognitive rehabilitation. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 12, 97–110.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding authors
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Stadie, N., van de Vijver, R. A linguistic and neuropsychological approach to remediation in a german case of developmental dysgraphia. Ann. of Dyslexia 53, 280–299 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-003-0013-5
Received:
Accepted:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-003-0013-5
Keywords
- Phonological Processing
- Dyslexia
- Correct Spelling
- Cognitive Neuropsychology
- Phonological Form