Advertisement

Springer Nature is making SARS-CoV-2 and COVID-19 research free. View research | View latest news | Sign up for updates

  • Part III Speech Development, Perception, and Production; Components of Reading; Defining Mathematics Learning Disability
  • Published:

Phonological awareness, speech development, and letter knowledge in preschool children

  • 800 Accesses

  • 35 Citations

Abstract

Phonological awareness has been shown to be one of the most reliable predictors and associates of reading ability. In an attempt to better understand its development, we have examined the interrelations of speech skills and letter knowledge to the phonological awareness and early reading skills of 99 preschool children. We found that phoneme awareness, but not rhyme awareness, correlated with early reading measures. We further found that phoneme manipulation was closely associated with letter knowledge and with letter sound knowledge, in particular, where rhyme awareness was closely linked with speech perception and vocabulary. Phoneme judgment fell in between. The overall pattern of results is consistent with phonological representation as an important factor in the complex relationship between preschool children’s phonological awareness, their emerging knowledge of the orthography, and their developing speech skills. However, where rhyme awareness is a concomitant of speech and vocabulary development, phoneme awareness more clearly associates with the products of literacy experience.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References

  1. Adams, M. R. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.

  2. Adlard, A., & Hazan, V. (1998). Speech perception in children with specific reading difficulties (dyslexia). The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51A, 153–177.

  3. Ball, E., & Blachman, B. (1991). Does phoneme awareness in kindergarten make a difference in early word recognition and developmental spelling? Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 49–66.

  4. Barron, R. W. (1998). Proto-literate knowledge: Antecedents and influences on phonological awareness and literacy. In C. Hulme & R. M. Joshi (Eds.). Reading and spelling: Development and disorders (pp. 153–175). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  5. Barron, R. W., Golden, J. O., Seldon, D. M., Tait, C. F., Marmurek, H. H. C., & Haines, L. C. (1992). Teaching prereading skills with a talking computer: Letter-sound knowledge and print feedback facilitate nonreaders’ phonological awareness training. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 4, 179–204.

  6. Barron, R. W. (1991) Proto-literacy, literacy, and the acquisition of phonological awareness. Learning and Individual Differences, 3, 243–255.

  7. Bertelson, P., de Gelder, B., Tfouni, L., & Morais, J. (1989). Metaphonological abilities of adult illiterates: New evidence for heterogeneity. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 1, 239–250.

  8. Bishop, D. V. M., & Adams, C. (1990). A prospective study of the relationship between specific language impairment, phonological disorders and reading retardation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Applied Disciplines, 31(7), 1027–1050.

  9. Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read - a causal connection. Nature, 301, 419–421.

  10. Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. (1991). Phonological skills before and after learning to read. In S. A. Brady & D. P. Shankweiler (Eds.). Phonological processes in literacy: A tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman (pp. 37–45). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  11. Brady, S., Poggie, E., & Rapala, M. M. (1989). Speech repetition abilities in children who differ in reading skill. Language and Speech, 32(2), 109–122.

  12. Brady, S., Shankweiler, D., & Mann, V. A. (1983). Speech perception and memory coding in relation to reading ability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 35, 345–367.

  13. Bryant, P. E., Bradley, L., MacLean, M., & Crossland, J. (1989). Nursery rhymes, phonological skills, and reading. Journal of Child Language, 16, 407–428.

  14. Burgess, S. R., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Bidirectional relations of phonological sensitivity and prereading abilities: Evidence from a preschool sample. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 70, 117–141.

  15. Byrne, B. (1996). The learnability of the alphabetic principle: Children’s initial hypotheses about how print represents spoken language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 17(4), 401–426.

  16. Byrne, B., & Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1990). Acquiring the alphabetic principle: A case for teaching recognition of phoneme identity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 805–812.

  17. Catts, H. (1986). Speech production/phonological deficits in reading-disordered children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 19, 504–508.

  18. Catts, H. (1991). Early identification of dyslexia: Evidence from a follow-up study of speech-language impaired children. Annals of Dyslexia, 41, 163–177.

  19. Chaney, C. (1992). Language development, metalinguistic skills, and print awareness in 3-year old children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 13, 485–514.

  20. Chiappe, P., Chiappe, D. L., & Siegel, L. (2001). Speech perception, lexicality and reading skill. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80, 58–74.

  21. Clay, M. (1979). The early detection of reading difficulties: A diagnostic survey with recovery procedures (2nd ed.). Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann Educational Books.

  22. Criddle, M. J., & Durkin, K. (2001). Phonological representation of novel morphemes in children with SLI and typically developing children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22(3), 363–382.

  23. Defior, S., & Tudela, P. (1994). Effect of phonological training on reading and writing acquisition. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 6, 299–320.

  24. Dietrich, J. A., & Brady, S. A. (2001). Phonological representations of adult poor readers: An investigation of specificity and stability. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22(3), 383–418.

  25. Ehri, L. C. (1983). A critique of five studies related to latter-name knowledge and learning to read. In L. Gentile, M. Kamil, & J. Blanchard (Eds.). Reading research: Advances in theory and practice: Vol. 1 (pp. 143–153). Columbus, OH: Merrill.

  26. Elbro, C. (1990). Differences in dyslexia. A study of reading strategies and deficits in a linguistic perspective. Copenhagen: Munksgaard.

  27. Elbro, C. (1996). Early linguistic abilities and reading development: A review and a hypothesis. Reading and Writing, 8, 453–485.

  28. Elbro, C., Borstrom, I., & Petersen, D. K. (1998). Predicting dyslexia from kindergarten: The importance of distinctness of phonological representations of lexical items. Reading Research Quarterly, 33(1), 36–60.

  29. Fowler, A. E. (1991). How early phonological development might set the stage for phonological awareness. In S. Brady & D. Shankweiler (Eds.). Phonological processes in literacy: A tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman (pp. 97–117). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  30. Foy, J. G., & Mann, V. A. (2001). Does strength of phonological representations predict phonological awareness in preschool children? Applied Psycholinguistics, 22(3), 301–325.

  31. Foy, J. G., & Mann, V. A. (2003). Home literacy environment and phonological awareness: Differential effects for phoneme awareness and rhyme awareness. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 59–88.

  32. Garlock, V. M., Walley, A. C., & Metsala, J. L. (2001). Age-of-acquisition, word frequency, and neighborhood density effects on spoken word recognition by children and adults. Journal of Memory and Language, 45(3), 468–492.

  33. Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C. S., Baddeley, A. D., & Emslie, H. (1994). The children’s test of nonword repetition: A test of phonological working memory. Memory. Special Issue: Memory tests and techniques, 2(2), 103–127.

  34. Godfrey, J. J., Syrdal-Lasky, A. K., Millay, K. K., & Knox, C. M. (1981). Performance of dyslexic children on speech perception tests. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 32, 401–424.

  35. Goldman, R., & Fristoe, M. (1986). Goldman-Fristoe test of articulation. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

  36. Goldman, R., Fristoe, M., & Woodcock, R. W. (1970). Test of auditory discrimination. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

  37. Gottardo A., Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. S. (1996). The relationships between phonological sensitivity, syntactic processing, and verbal working memory in the reading performance of third-grade children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63(3), 563–582.

  38. Hulme, C. (2002). Phonemes, rimes, and the mechanisms of early reading development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 58–64.

  39. Hulme, C., Hatcher, P. J., Nation, K., Brown, A., Adams, J., & Stuart, G. (2002). Phoneme awareness is a better predictor of early reading skill than onset-rime awareness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 2–28.

  40. Joanisse, M. F., Manis, F. R., Keating, P., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2000). Language deficits in dyslexic children: Speech perception, phonology and morphology, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77(1), 30–60.

  41. Lyon, G. R. (1995). Toward a definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 45, 3–27.

  42. Manis, F. R., McBride-Chang, C., Seidenberg, M. S., Keating, P., Doi, L. M., & Petersen, A. (1997). Are speech perception deficits associated with developmental dyslexia? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 66, 211–235.

  43. Mann, V. A. (1984). Longitudinal prediction and prevention of early reading difficulty. Annals of Dyslexia, 34, 117–136.

  44. Mann, V. A. (1993). Phoneme awareness and future reading ability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26, 259–269.

  45. Mann, V. A. (1998). Language problems: A key to early reading problems. In B. Y. L. Wong (Ed.). Learning about learning disabilities (2nd ed.) (pp. 163–201). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

  46. Mann, V. A., Tobin, P., & Wilson, R. (1988). Measuring phonological awareness through the invented spelling of kindergarten children. In K. E. Stanovich (Ed.). Children’s reading and the development of phonological awareness (pp. 121–147). Detroit, MI: Wayne State University Press

  47. Mann, V. A., & Wimmer, H. (2002). Phoneme awareness and pathways into literacy: A comparison of German and American children. Reading and Writing, 15, 653–682.

  48. Marshall, C. M., Snowling, M. J., & Bailey, P. J. (2001). Rapid auditory processing and phonological ability in normal readers and readers with dyslexia. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44(4), 925–941.

  49. Metsala, J. L., & Walley, A. C. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In J. L. Metsala & L. C. Ehri (Eds.). Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 89–120). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  50. Morais, J. (1991a). Constraints on the development of phonemic awareness. In S. A. Brady & D. P. Shankweiler (Eds.). Phonological processes in literacy: A tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman (pp. 5–27). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  51. Morais, J. (1991b). Phonological awareness: A bridge between language and literacy. In D. J. Sawyer & B. J. Fox (Eds.). Phonological awareness in reading: The evolution of current perspectives (pp. 31–71). New York: Springer Verlag.

  52. Morais, J., Bertelson, P., Cary, L., & Alegria, J. (1986). Literacy training and speech segmentation. Cognition, 24, 45–64.

  53. Morais, J., Cary, L., Alegria, J., & Bertelson, P. (1979). Does awareness of speech as a sequence of phones arise spontaneously? Cognition, 7, 323–331.

  54. Muter, V. (1994). The influence of phonological awareness and letter knowledge on beginning reading and spelling development. In C. Hulme & M. Snowling (Eds.). Reading development and dyslexia (pp. 45–62). London: Whurr.

  55. Muter, V., & Snowling, M. (1998). Concurrent and longitudinal predictors of reading: The role of metalinguistic and short-term memory skills. Reading Research Quarterly, 33(3), 320–337.

  56. National Reading Panel. (2000). Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction. (NIH Publication No. 00-4769). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.

  57. Nittrouer, S. (1999). Do temporal processing deficits cause phonological processing problems? Journal of Speech, Language, & Hearing Research, 42(4), 925–942.

  58. Read, C. A., Zhang, Y., Nie, H., & Ding, B. (1986). The ability to manipulate speech sounds depends on knowing the alphabetic reading. Cognition, 24, 31–44.

  59. Serniclaes, W., Sprenger-Charolles, L., Carre, R., & Demonet, J.-F. (2001). Perceptual discrimination of speech sounds in developmental dyslexia. Journal of Speech, Language, & Hearing Research, 44(2), 384–398.

  60. Silva, P. A., Williams, S. M., & McGee, R. (1987). A longitudinal study of children with developmental language delay at age three: Later intelligence, reading and behavior problems. Developmental Medicine and Neurology, 29, 630–640.

  61. Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (Eds.). (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children/Committee on the prevention of reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

  62. Snowling, M. (1981). Phonemic deficits in developmental dyslexia. Psychological Research, 43, 219–234.

  63. Snowling, M., & Hulme, C. (1994). The development of phonological skills. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 41B, 21–28.

  64. Snowling, M., Goulandris, N., Bowlby, M., & Howell, P. (1986). Segmentation and speech perception in relation to reading skill: A developmental analysis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 41, 489–507.

  65. Snowling, M., Hulme, C., Smith, A., & Thomas, J. (1994). The effects of phonemic similarity and list length on children’s sound categorisation performance. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 58, 160–180.

  66. Stanovich, K. E. (1992). Speculations on the causes and consequences of individual differences in the early reading acquisition. In P. B. Gough, L. C. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.). Reading acquisition (pp. 307–342). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  67. Stanovich, K. E. (1994). Does dyslexia exist? Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 35(4), 579–595.

  68. Stothard, S. E., Snowling, M. J., Bishop, D. V. M., Chipcase, B. B., & Kaplan, C. A. (1998). Language-impaired preschoolers: A follow-up into adolescence. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 407–418.

  69. Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

  70. Torgesen, J. K., & Davis, C. (1996). Individual difference variables that predict response to training in phonological awareness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 1–21.

  71. Treiman, R. (1993). Beginning to spell: A study of first grade children. New York: Oxford University Press.

  72. Treiman, R. (1998). Why spelling? The benefits of incorporating spelling into beginning reading instruction. In J. L. Ehri & L. C. Metsala (Eds). Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 289–313). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  73. Treiman, R., & Broderick, V. (1998). What’s in a name: Children’s knowledge about the letters in their own name. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 70, 97–116.

  74. Treiman, R., Tincoff, R., Rodriguez, K., Mouzaki, A., & Francis, D. J. (1998). The foundations of literacy: Learning the sounds of letters. Child Development, 69, 1524–1540.

  75. Treiman, R., Zukowski, A., & Richmond-Welty, E. D. (1995). What happened to the “n” of sink? Children’s spellings of final consonant clusters. Cognition, 55, 1–38.

  76. Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 30, 73–87.

  77. Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bi-directional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 36–39.

  78. Walley, A. C. (1993). The role of vocabulary development in children’s spoken word recognition and segmentation ability. Developmental Review, 13, 286–350.

  79. Wechsler, D. (1992). Wechsler intelligence scale for children. London: Psychological Corporation.

  80. Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (1987). Speech perception in severely disabled and average reading children. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 41, 48–61.

  81. Wolf, M. (1991). Naming speed and reading: The contribution of the cognitive neurosciences. Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 123–141.

  82. Woodcock, R. M. (1987). Woodcock reading mastery - Revised, Form H. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

  83. Worden, P. E., & Boettcher, W. (1990). Young children’s acquisition of alphabet knowledge. Journal of Reading Behavior, 22, 277–295.

Download references

Author information

Correspondence to Virginia A. Mann.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Mann, V.A., Foy, J.G. Phonological awareness, speech development, and letter knowledge in preschool children. Ann. of Dyslexia 53, 149–173 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-003-0008-2

Download citation

Keywords

  • Phonological Awareness
  • Speech Perception
  • Word Reading
  • Digit Span
  • Poor Reader