Skip to main content
Log in

Is dyslexia a form of specific language impairment? a comparison of dyslexic and language impaired children as adolescents

  • Part III Language Development And Reading Disabilities
  • Published:
Annals of Dyslexia Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Two groups of adolescents with a childhood history of language impairment were compared with a group of developmentally dyslexic young people of the same age and nonverbal ability. The study also included two comparison groups of typically developing children, one of the same age as those in the clinical groups, and a younger comparison group of similar reading level to the dyslexic students. Tests of spoken and written language skills revealed that the adolescents with dyslexia were indistinguishable from those with resolved language impairments on spoken language tasks, and both groups performed at age-expected levels. However, both dyslexic readers and those with resolved specific language impairments showed deficits in phonological awareness. On written language tasks, a different pattern of performance was apparent. In reading and spelling, adolescents with dyslexia performed only as well as those with persistent oral language impairments and younger controls. However, their reading comprehension was better. The theoretical and educational implications of these findings are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • American Psychiatric Association. 1994. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders IV. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aram, D., Ekelman, B., and Nation, J. 1984. Preschoolers with language disorders: 10 years later. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 27:232–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M. 1997. Uncommon Understanding. Hove: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M., and Edmundson, A. 1987. Language impaired 4-year-olds: Distinguishing transient from persistent impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 52:156–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M., and Leonard, L. C. 2000. Speech and Language Impairments: From Research to Practice. Hove: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M., North, T., and Donlan, C. 1996. Nonword repetition as a behavioural marker for inherited language impairment: Evidence from a twin study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 37:391–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, B., Fielding-Barnsley, R., Ashley, L., and Larsen, K. 1997. Assessing the child’s and the environment’s contribution to reading acquisition: What we know and what we don’t know. In Foundations of Reading Acquisition and Dyslexia: Implications for Early Intervention, B. Blachman, ed. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W. 1989. Defining dyslexia as a developmental language disorder. Annals of Dyslexia 39:50–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W. 1993. The relationship between speech language and reading disabilities. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 36:948–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W. 1996. Defining dyslexia as a developmental language disorder: An expanded view. Topics in Language Disorders 16(2):14–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., Whetton, C., and Pintilie, D. 1982. British Picture Vocabulary Scale. Windsor, England: NFER-Nelson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., Baddeley, A. D., and Emslie, H. 1994. The children’s test of nonword repetition: A test of phonological working memory. Memory 2:103–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kamhi, A. G., and Catts, H. W. 1986. Toward an understanding of developmental language and reading disorders. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 5:337–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leonard, L., Sabbatini, L., Leonard, J., and Volterra, V. 1987. Specific language impairment in children: A cross-linguistic study. Brain and Language 32:233–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McKenna, R., and Warrington, E. K. 1983. Graded Naming Test. Windsor, England: NFER-Nelson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., and Snowling, M. J. 1998. Individual differences in contextual facilitation: Evidence from dyslexia and poor reading comprehension. Child Development 69:996–1011.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perin, D. 1983. Phonemic segmentation and spelling. British Journal of Psychology 74:245–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plaut, D. C., McClelland, J. L., Seidenberg, M. S., and Patterson, K. 1996. Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains. Psychological Review 103:56–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rack, J., Snowling, M., and Olson, R. 1992. The non-word reading deficit in dyslexia: A review. Reading Research Quarterly 27:28–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., and Yule, W. 1975. The concept of specific reading retardation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 16:181–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H. 1990. Very early language deficits in dyslexic children. Child Development 61:1728–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H., and Dobrich, W. 1990. Development of children with early language delay. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 33:70–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Semel, E., Wiig, E. K., and Secord, W. 1986. The Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-Revised. New York: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. 1995. Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition 55:151–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaywitz, S. E., Escobar, M. D., Shaywitz, B. A., Fletcher, J. M., and Makugh, R. 1992. Evidence that dyslexia may represent the lower tail of a normal distribution of reading ability. New England Journal of Medicine 326:145–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J. 1991. Developmental reading disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 32:49–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J. 1996. Annotation: Contemporary approaches to the teaching of reading. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 37:139–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J. 2000. Language and literacy skills. Who is at risk and why? In Speech and Language Impairments: From Research to Practice, D. V. M. Bishop and L. C. Leonard, eds. Hove: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J., Bishop, D. V. M., and Stothard, S. E. 2000. Is pre-school language impairment a risk factor for dyslexia in adolescence? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 41, 587–600.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J., Goulandris, N., and Defty, N. 1996. A longitudinal study of reading development in dyslexic children. Journal of Educational Psychology 88(40):653–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J., Stothard, S. E., and MacLean, J. 1996. Graded Nonword Reading Test. Bury St. Edmunds, England: Thames Valley Test Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M., Van Wagtendonk, B., and Stafford, C. 1988. Object naming deficits in developmental dyslexia. Journal of Research in Reading 11(2):67–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stackhouse, J., and Wells, B. 1997. Children’s Speech and Literacy Difficulties. London: Whurr Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., and Siegel, L. S. 1994. The phenotypic performance profile of reading-disabled children: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Educational Psychology 86:24–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stothard, S. E., Snowling, M. J., Bishop, D. V. M., Chipchase, B., and Kaplan, C. 1998. Language impaired pre-schoolers: A follow-up in adolescence. Journal of Speech, Hearing and Language Research 41:407–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tabachnick, G., and Fidell, L. 1996. Using Multivariate Statistics (3rd ed.). New York: Harper Collins College Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tallal, P., Ross, R., and Curtiss, S. 1989. Family aggregation in specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Hearing and Language Research 27:987–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van der Lely, H., and Stollwerk, L. 1996. A grammatical specific language impairment in children. An autosomal dominant inheritance? Brain and Language 52:484–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. 1992. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children - Third Edition. UK. New York: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler D. 1993. The Wechsler Objective Reading Dimensions (WORD). New York: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., Mayringer, H., and Landerl, K. 1998. Poor reading: A deficit in skill-automatization or a phonological deficit? Scientific Studies of Reading 2:321–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., and Bowers, P. G. 1999. The double deficit hypothesis for developmental dyslexia. Journal of Educational Psychology 91:154–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Nata K. Goulandris, Margaret J. Snowling or Ian Walker.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Goulandris, N.K., Snowling, M.J. & Walker, I. Is dyslexia a form of specific language impairment? a comparison of dyslexic and language impaired children as adolescents. Annals of Dyslexia 50, 103–120 (2000). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-000-0019-1

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-000-0019-1

Keywords

Navigation