Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

An interactionist perspective on mathematics learning: conditions of learning opportunities in mixed-ability groups within linguistic negotiation processes

  • Original Article
  • Published:
ZDM Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper illustrates that focusing on processes of interaction is crucial to a closer understanding of mathematical learning processes in mixed-ability groups. In doing so, the paper’s focus is on expounding a theoretical-methodological framework of an interactionist perspective in mathematics education. This framework interlinks sociological and social-constructivist theories with subject-specific educational theories. As a result of two examples of analyses it becomes apparent that investigations on mathematics learning within linguistic negotiation processes, based on an interactionist-oriented theoretical frame, provide in-depth insights into individual learning possibilities of a diverse student body. These insights are made possible only by means of a detailed micro-sociological examination of collectively occurring learning processes within linguistic negotiation processes.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Both transcripts are included in the original language (German) in the electronic supplementary material of this paper. The rules for transcription are presented in the “Appendix”.

  2. Following Jensen (2003), subtractions can be done in two ways: “comparative subtraction” and “take-away subtraction”. In German references, there is a third category and the mental models (vom Hofe et al. 2006) of subtraction can be distinguished into: taking-away, supplementing and comparing (Wessel 2015). Thus, we use these terms. The mental models of taking-away and supplementing have a dynamic characteristic. Taking-away is characterized by taking away the subtrahend from the minuend. For supplementing, the gap between the minuend (or the sum) and the subtrahend (or the first summand) is determined by an (imaginary) action. On the contrary, comparing is static and is characterized by the gap between the two given items without performing the action of taking away or supplementing. According to the interactionist understanding at hand, these basic notions are used in analyses to differentiate the learners’ framings regarding subtraction, and to visualize modulations in framings.

References

  • Austin, J. L., & Howson, A. G. (1979). Language and mathematical education. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 10, 161–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barwell, R. (2003). Discursive psychology and mathematics education: Possibilities and challenges. ZDM, 35(5), 201–207.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauersfeld, H. (1985). Ergebnisse und Probleme von Mikroanalysen mathematischen Unterrichts. In W. Dörfler & R. Fischer (Eds.), Empirische Untersuchungen zum Lehren und Lernen von Mathematik (pp. 7–25). Wien: Hölder-Pichler-Tempsky.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauersfeld, H., Krummheuer, G., & Voigt, J. (1988). Interactional theory of learning and teaching mathematics and related microethnographical studies. In H.-G. Steiner, & A. Vermandel (Eds.), Foundations and methodology of the discipline mathematics education (pp. 174–188). Antwerp: University of Antwerp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blumer, H. (1969). Symbolic interactionism. Perspective and method. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bottge, B. A., Heinrichs, M., Mehta, Z. D., & Hung, Y. (2002). Weighing the benefits of anchored math instruction for students with disabilities in general education classes. Journal of Special Education, 35, 186–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1983). Childs’s talk. Learning to use language. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doise, W., & Mugny, G. (1979). Individual and collective conflicts of centration in cognitive development. European Journal of Social Psychology, 9, 245–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garfinkel, H. (1967). Studies in ethnomethodology. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1974). Frame analysis. An essay on the organisation of experience. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1981). Forms of talk. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingram, J. (2018). Moving forward with ethnomethodological approaches to analysing mathematics classroom interactions. ZDM. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11858-018-0951-3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, G. R. (2003). Arithmetic for teachers. With applications and topics from geometry. Providence. Rhode Island: American Mathematical Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krummheuer, G. (1992). Lernen mit »Format«. Elemente einer interaktionistischen Lerntheorie. Diskutiert an Beispielen mathematischen Unterrichts. Weinheim: Deutscher Studien Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krummheuer, G. (1995). The ethnography of argumentation. In P. Cobb & H. Bauersfeld (Eds.), The emergence of mathematical meaning (pp. 229–270). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krummheuer, G. (2007). Argumentation and participation in the primary mathematics classroom: Two episodes and related theoretical abductions. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 26, 60–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krummheuer, G., & Brandt, B. (2001). Paraphrase und Traduktion. Weinheim: Beltz Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kunsch, C. A., Jitendra, A. K., & Sood, S. (2007). The effects of peer-mediated instruction in mathematics for students with learning problems: A research synthesis. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 22, 1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Langner, A., & Schütte, M. (2015). Teilhabe an Bildung von Anfang an. In U. Mahnke, H. Redlich, L. Schäfer, G. Wachtel, V. Moser & K. Zehbe (Eds.), Tagungsband: Perspektiven sonderpädagogischer Professionalisierung (pp. 273–281). Bad Heilbrunn: Klinkhardt Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerman, S. (2000). The social turn in mathematics education research. In J. Boaler (Ed.), Multiple perspectives on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 19–44). Westport: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, M. (1986). Kollektive Lernprozesse. Studien zur Grundlegung einer soziologischen Lerntheorie. Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, C., Craig, T., Schütte, M., & Wagner, D. (2014). Language and communication in mathematics education: An overview of research in the field. ZDMThe International Journal on Mathematics Education, 46(6), 843–853.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moschkovich, J. (2002). A situated and sociocultural perspective on bilingual mathematics learners. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 4(2), 189–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moschkovich, J. N. (2015). Scaffolding mathematical practices. ZDM Mathematics Education, 47(7), 1067–1078.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nührenbörger, M. (2010). Einsichtsvolles Mathematiklernen im Kontext von Heterogenität. In A. Lindmeier & S. Ufer (Eds.), Beiträge zum Mathematikunterricht (pp. 641–644). Münster: WTM-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Planas, N. (2018). Language as resource: A key notion for understanding the complexity of mathematics learning. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 87(1), 51–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Planas, N., & Civil, M. (2013). Language-as-resource and language-as-political: tensions in the bilingual mathematics classroom. Mathematics Education Research Journal, 25(3), 361–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schütte, M. (2014). Language-related specialised learning in mathematics. ZDM—The International Journal on Mathematics Education, 46(6), 923–938.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schütte, M. (2018). Subject-specific academic language versus mathematical discourse. In J. N. Moschkovich, D. Wagner, A. Bose, J. Rodrigues Mendes & M. Schütte (Eds.), Language and communication in mathematics education (pp. 25–36). Cham: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Schütte, M., & Krummheuer, G. (2017). Mathematische Diskurse im Kindesalter—Der narratorische Diskurs. In Institut für Mathematik der Universität Potsdam (Eds.), Beiträge zum Mathematikunterricht 2017 (pp. 877–880). Münster: WTM-Verlag.

  • Schwarz, B., Neumann, Y., & Biezuner (2000). Two wrongs may make a right… if they argue together! Cognition and Instruction, 18(4), 461–494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarzkopf, R. (2000). Argumentationsprozesse im Mathematikunterricht. Hildesheim: Franzbecker.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sfard, A. (2008). Thinking as communicating: Human development, the growth of discourses and mathematizing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, Y. (2009). Mathematical literacy: Developing identities of inclusion. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toulmin, S. E. (2003). The uses of argument. Updated edition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • vom Hofe, R., Kleine, M., Blum, W., & Pekrun, R. (2006). The effect of mental models for the development of mathematical competencies. In M. Bosch (Ed.), European research in mathematics education (pp. 142–151). Llull: IQS.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wessel, J. (2015). Grundvorstellungen und Vorgehensweisen bei der Subtraktion. Wiesbaden: Springer Spektrum.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marcus Schütte.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (PDF 91 KB)

Appendix

Appendix

1.1 Transcript conventions

<

Overlapping/simultaneous speech

#

Interrupting, seamless transition between speakers

. / .. / ..

Pause for one to three seconds

smaller

Spoken quietly, whisper

n o w

Elongation

/

Strong rising intonation

(but)

Word not fully comprehensible

(incomprehensible)

Incomprehensible utterance

[stands up]

Actions, facial expressions, gestures, paralinguistic utterances

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Jung, J., Schütte, M. An interactionist perspective on mathematics learning: conditions of learning opportunities in mixed-ability groups within linguistic negotiation processes. ZDM Mathematics Education 50, 1089–1099 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11858-018-0999-0

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11858-018-0999-0

Keywords

Navigation