ZDM

, Volume 47, Issue 3, pp 451–463 | Cite as

Children’s understanding of large-scale mapping tasks: an analysis of talk, drawings, and gesture

  • Donna Kotsopoulos
  • Michelle Cordy
  • Melanie Langemeyer
Original Article

Abstract

This research examined how children represent motion in large-scale mapping tasks that we referred to as “motion maps”. The underlying mathematical content was transformational geometry. In total, 19 children, 8- to 10-year-old, created motion maps and captured their motion maps with accompanying verbal description digitally. Analysis of the responses included a fine-grained coding of their drawing, oral description, and hand gestures used while describing. In addition, the classroom teacher (second author) also assessed the drawings and the oral responses. Results indicate that low achieving children produced fewer objects in their drawings, fewer gestures, and fewer verbal descriptions when engaging in a large-scale mapping task compared to high achieving children. Moreover, these children were found to use a series of connected small-scale maps to construct the larger-scale representation.

Keywords

Children Drawing Dynamic Gesture Map Spatial Static Transformational geometry Verbal 

References

  1. Alibali, M. W., & DiRusso, A. A. (1999). The function of gesture in learning to count: More than keeping track. Cognitive Development, 14(1), 37–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Anderson, K. L., Casey, M. B., Thompson, W. L., Burrage, M. S., Pezaris, E., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2008). Performance on middle school geometry problems with geometry clues matched to three different cognitive styles. Mind, Brain, and Education, 2(4), 188–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Botzer, G., & Yerushalmy, M. (2008). Embodied semiotic activities and their role in the construction of mathematical meaning of motion graphs. International Journal of Computers for Mathematical Learning, 13(2), 111–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Butterworth, G. (2003). Pointing is the royal road to language in babies. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet (pp. 85–107). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  5. Chang, K.-E., Sung, Y.-T., Chen, Y.-L., & Huang, L.-H. (2008). Learning multiplication through computer-assisted learning activities. Computers in Human Behavior, 24(6), 2904–2916.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Cheng, K., Huttenlocher, J., & Newcombe, N. S. (2013). 25 years of research on the use of geometry in spatial reorientation: A current theoretical perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 20(6), 1033–1054.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Clearfield, M. W. (2004). The role of crawling and walking experience in infant spatial memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 89(3), 214–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Clements, D. H. (2004). Geometric and spatial thinking in early childhood education. In D. H. Clements, J. Sarama, & A.-M. Di Biase (Eds.), Engaging young children in mathematics: Standards for early childhood mathematics education (pp. 267–298). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates Inc.Google Scholar
  9. Cook, S. W., Mitchell, Z., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2008). Gesturing makes learning last. Cognition, 106(2), 1047–1058.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. DeLoache, J. S. (1987). Rapid change in the symbolic functioning of very young children. Science, 238(4833), 1556–1557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. DeLoache, J. S. (1989). Young children’s understanding of the correspondence between a scale model and a larger space. Cognitive Development, 4(2), 121–139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Devichi, C., & Munier, V. (2013). About the concept of angle in elementary school: Misconceptions and teaching sequences. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 32(1), 1–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Ehrlich, S. B., Levine, S. C., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2006). The importance of gesture in children’s spatial reasoning. Developmental Psychology, 42(6), 1259–1268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Emmorey, K., Tversky, B., & Taylor, H. A. (2000). Using space to describe space: Perspective in speech, sign, and gesture. Spatial Cognition and Computation, 2(3), 157–180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Erbas, A. K., & Yenmez, A. A. (2011). The effect of inquiry-based explorations in a dynamic geometry environment on sixth grade students’ achievements in polygons. Computers and Education, 57(4), 2462–2475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Ferrara, K., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Newcombe, N. S., Golinkoff, R., & Lam, W. S. (2011). Block talk: Spatial language during block play. Mind, Brain and Education, 5(3), 143–151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Feyereisen, P., & Havard, I. (1999). Mental imagery and production of hand gestures while speaking in younger and older adults. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 23(2), 153–171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Frick, A., & Newcombe, N. S. (2012). Getting the big picture: Development of spatial scaling abilities. Cognitive Development, 27(3), 270–282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gagatsis, A., & Shiakalli, M. (2004). Ability to translate from one representation of the concept of function to another and mathematical problem solving. Educational Psychology, 24(5), 645–657.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Göksun, T., Goldin-Meadow, S., Newcombe, N., & Shipley, T. (2013). Individual differences in mental rotation: What does gesture tell us? Cognitive Processes, 14(2), 153–162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Holden, C. (2006). Hunter-gatherers grasp geometry. Science, 311(5759), 317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Izard, V., & Spelke, E. S. (2009). Development of sensitivity to geometry in visual forms. Human Evolution, 23(3), 213–248.Google Scholar
  23. Jacobson, C., & Lehrer, R. (2000). Teacher appropriation and student learning of geometry through design. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 31(1), 71–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Jones, K. (2000). Providing a foundation for deductive reasoning: Students’ interpretations when using dynamic geometry software and their evolving mathematical explanations. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 44(1/2), 55–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Kendrick, M., & McKay, R. (2004). Drawings as an alternative way of understanding young children’s constructions of literacy. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 4(1), 109–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Krauss, R. M. (1998). Why do we gesture when we speak? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7(2), 54–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Levine, M., Jankovic, I. N., & Pahj, M. (1982). Principles of spatial problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 111(2), 157–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Mammarella, I. C., Giofrè, D., Ferrara, R., & Cornoldi, C. (2012). Intuitive geometry and visuospatial working memory in children showing symptoms of nonverbal learning disabilities. Child Neuropsychology, 19(3), 235–249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Martínez, J. F., Stecher, B., & Borko, H. (2009). Classroom assessment practices, teacher judgments, and student achievement in mathematics: Evidence from the ECLS. Educational Assessment, 14, 78–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. McGillicuddy-de Lisi, A. V., & de Lisi, R. (1981). Children’s strategies in imagining spatio-geometrical transformations. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 4(2), 201–222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Möhring, W., Newcombe, N.S., & Frick, A. (2014). Zooming in on spatial scaling: Preschool children and adults use mental transformations to scale spaces. Developmental Psychology, 50(5), 1614–1619.Google Scholar
  32. Ontario Ministry of Education. (2010). Growing success: Assessment, evaluation, and reporting in Ontario schools. First edition, covering grades 1 to 12. Toronto: Queen’s Printer for Ontario.Google Scholar
  33. Ontario Ministry of Education and Training/OMET. (2005). The Ontario curriculum grades 1–8 mathematics, revised. Toronto: Queen’s Printer for Ontario.Google Scholar
  34. Ping, R. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2008). Hands in the air: Using ungrounded iconic gestures to teach children conservation of quantity. Developmental Psychology, 44(5), 1277–1287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Pruden, S. M., Levine, S. C., & Huttenlocher, J. (2011). Children’s spatial thinking: Does talk about the spatial world matter? Developmental Science, 14(6), 1417–1430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Quaiser-Pohl, C., Lehmann, W., & Eid, M. (2004). The relationship between spatial abilities and representations of large-scale space in children–a structural equation modeling analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 36(1), 95–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Radford, L. (2009). “No! He starts walking backwards!”: Interpreting motion graphs and the question of space, place and distance. ZDM - The International Journal on Mathematics Education, 41(4), 467–480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Saads, S. M. L., & Edwards, C. W. (1997). Understanding shapes and space from reflection and rotation. International Journal of Mathematical Education in Science and Technology, 28(3), 437–446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Sauter, M., Uttal, D. H., Alman, A. S., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Levine, S. C. (2012). Learning what children know about space from looking at their hands: The added value of gesture in spatial communication. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111(4), 587–606.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Shusterman, A., Ah Lee, S., & Spelke, E. S. (2011). Cognitive effects of language on human navigation. Cognition, 120(2), 186–201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Sinclair, N., Healy, L., & Sales, C. O. R. (2009). Time for telling stories: Narrative thinking with dynamic geometry. ZDM - The International Journal on Mathematics Education, 41(4), 441–452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Sinclair, N., & Moss, J. (2012). The more it changes, the more it becomes the same: The development of the routine of shape identification in dynamic geometry environment. International Journal of Educational Research, 51–52, 28–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Smith, T., & MacDonald, A. (2009). Time for talk: The drawing-telling process. Australian Primary Mathematics Classroom, 14(3), 21–26.Google Scholar
  44. Spelke, E. S., Lee, S. A., & Izard, V. (2010). Beyond core knowledge: Natural geometry. Cognitive Science, 34(5), 863–884.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Tunteler, E., Pronk, C. M. E., & Resing, W. C. M. (2008). Inter- and intra-individual variability in the process of change in the use of analogical strategies to solve geometric tasks in children: A microgenetic analysis. Learning and Individual Differences, 18(1), 44–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Uttal, D. H. (1996). Angles and distances: Children’s and adults’ reconstruction and scaling of spatial configurations. Child Development, 67(6), 2763–2779.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Uttal, D. H. (2000). Seeing the big picture: Map use and the development of spatial cognition. Developmental Science, 3(3), 286–747.Google Scholar
  48. Uttal, D. H., Meadow, N. G., Tipton, E., Hand, L. L., Alden, A. R., Warren, C., et al. (2013). The malleability of spatial skills: A meta-analysis of training studies. Psychological Bulletin, 139(2), 352–402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Vasilyeva, M., & Huttenlocher, J. (2004). Early development of scaling ability. Developmental Psychology, 40(5), 682–690.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Woleck, K. R. (2001). Listen to their pictures: An investigation of children’s mathematical drawings. In A. A. Cuoco & F. R. Curcio (Eds.), The roles of representation in school mathematics (pp. 215–227). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© FIZ Karlsruhe 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  • Donna Kotsopoulos
    • 1
    • 2
  • Michelle Cordy
    • 3
  • Melanie Langemeyer
    • 4
    • 5
  1. 1.Faculty of EducationWilfrid Laurier UniversityWaterlooCanada
  2. 2.Faculty of ScienceWilfrid Laurier UniversityWaterlooCanada
  3. 3.Thames Valley District School BoardLondonCanada
  4. 4.Georgian CollegeBarrieCanada
  5. 5.University of TorontoTorontoCanada

Personalised recommendations