Skip to main content

Optimal stimulation level theory, exploratory consumer behaviour and product adoption: an analysis of underlying structures across product categories

Abstract

Research of the last decades focused on answering several questions in view of optimum stimulation theory. Which variables do influence general explorative tendencies such as risk taking, variety seeking, or curiosity-motivated behaviour and how do the general explorative tendencies effect the willingness to choose new or familiar products on a repurchase occasion? The approach of this research is the attempt to analyse a wide range of variables, which have successfully been tested to influence innovative behaviour, within one simultaneous model. This research is applied across different product-categories. It is confirming the nomological validity of OSL-theory including some contextual variables by attesting a principal consistency between all (six) models that have been established throughout the range of several examined product categories. The parameters between inherent factors do not contradict when comparing the single models with each other, parameter values do only differ slightly according to category-specific peculiarities. As a consequence of these results, an overall fairly good picture is drawn of what is behind the keenness for innovations of early adopters in general (regardless of the product category).

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.

Fig. 1

Notes

  1. Guido et al. (2007) found significant correlations (p < 0.05) between OSL and Agreeableness (0.847), Introversion (−0.249) and Openness to Experience (0.611). Nicholson et al. (2005) even found significant (p < 0.05) relationships between all personality factors and overall risk propensity (extraversion and openness positively associated; neuroticism, agreeableness, and conscientiousness negatively associated with risk propensity).

  2. The ADF-estimations (asymptotically distribution-free methods) can as well be used regardless of underlying distributions but has some serious problems in practise. Sample sizes must exceed at least 1,000 cases and the models need to be small. Also, they do not necessarily perform better. ADF techniques are rather seen to be theoretically interesting but not an appropriate practical alternative (e.g. Baumgartner and Homburg 1996, p. 149).

References

  • Bagozzi RP, Yi Y (1988) On the evaluation of structural equation models. J Acad Mark Sci 16(1):74–94

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Balderjahn I (1986) The robustness of LISREL unweighted least squares estimation against small sample size in confirmatory factor analysis models. In: Gaul W, Schader M (eds) Classification as a tool of research. Proceedings of the 9th annual meeting of the classification society. pp 3–10

  • Bänsch A (1995) Variety Seeking—Marketingfolgerungen aus Überlegungen und Untersuchungen zum Abwechslungsbedürfnis von Konsumenten. Jahrbuch der Absatz- und Verbrauchsforschung 41(4):342–365

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumgartner H, Homburg C (1996) Applications of structural equation modeling in marketing and consumer research: a review. Int J Res Mark 13:139–161

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumgartner H, Steenkamp J-BEM (1996) Exploratory Consumer Buying Behavior, Conceptualization and Measurement. Int J Res Mark 13:121–137

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne DE (1960) Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. McGraw-Hill, New York

  • Browne MW, Cudeck R (1993) Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In: Bollen KA, Long SJ (eds) Testing structural equation models. Sage, Newbury Park, pp 445–455

  • Burns DJ, Krampf RF (1992) Explaining innovative behavior: uniqueness-seeking and sensation-seeking. Int J Advert 11(3):227–337

    Google Scholar 

  • Chin WW, Newsted PR (1999) Structural equation modeling analysis with small samples using partial least squares. In: Hoyle RH (ed) Strategies for small sample research. Sage Thousand Oaks, CA, pp 307–341

  • Chuang SC, Kung C-Y (2005) The effects of emotions in risk-taking. J Am Acad Bus Camb 2:113–117

    Google Scholar 

  • Daghfous N, Petrof JV, Pons F (1999) Values and adoption of innovations: a cross-cultural study. J Consum Mark 16(4):314–331

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Digman JM (1990) Personality structure: emergence of the five-factor model. Annu Rev Psychol 41(1):417–440

    Google Scholar 

  • Etzel MJ, Wahlers RG (1984) Optimal stimulation level and consumer travel preferences. In: 1984 American Marketing Association Educators Proceeding. Chicago, pp 92–95

  • Fiore AM, Lee S-E, Kunz G (2002) Individual differences, motivations, and willingness to mass customization option for fashion products. Eur J Mark 38/7:835–849

    Google Scholar 

  • Fornell C, Bookstein FL (1982) Two structural equation models: LISREL and PLS applied to consumer exit-voice theory. J Mark Res 19/11:440–452

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garlington WK, Shimota HE (1964) The change seeker index: a measure of the need for variable stimulus input. Psychol Rep 14:919–924

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerbing DW, Anderson JC (1993) Monte Carlo evaluations of goodness-of-fit indices for structural equation models. In: Bollen KA, Long SJ (eds) Testing structural equation models. Sage, Newbury Park, pp 40–65

  • Gierl H, Helm R, Stumpp S (1999) Erklärung des Konsumentenverhaltens durch die Optimum Stimulation Level Theorie. Marketing ZFP, vol 3, 3rd quarter, pp 217–235

  • Gierl H, Helm R, Praxmarer S, Stumpp S (2000) Konsequenzen für das Marketing aus dem Explorationsstreben der Konsumenten. Die Unternehmung 54(5):331–349

    Google Scholar 

  • Gierl H, Helm R, Stumpp S (2002) Markentreue und Kaufintervalle bei langlebigen Konsumgütern. Zeitschrift für betriebswirtschaftlich Forschung 54:215–232

    Google Scholar 

  • Givon M (1984) Variety seeking through brand switching. Mark Sci 3:1–22

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldsmith RE, Flynn LR, Goldsmith EB (2003) Innovative consumers and market mavens. J Market 11(4):54–64

    Google Scholar 

  • Grande I (2005) Dimensions in scales for measuring exploratory tendencies and stimulation levels in consumers: a cross-cultural comparison of the USA and Spain. J Consum Behav 4:363–373

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grob A (1995) A structural model of environmental attitudes and behavior. J Environ Psychol 15:209–220

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guido G, Capestro M, Peluso AM (2007) Experimental analysis of consumer stimulation and motivational states in shopping experiences. Int J Mark Res 49/3:365–386

    Google Scholar 

  • Hasselmann H, Helm R (2001) Exploratory consumer buying behaviour: how to identify innovator characteristics? In: Marketing transformation: research for the bottom line. Esomar Publication Series, Rome, pp 113–129

  • Helm R (2001) Planung und Vermarktung von Innovationen, Stuttgart

  • Herrmann A, Huber F, Kressmann F (2006) Varianz- und kovarianzbasierte Strukturgleichungsmodelle–Ein Leitfaden zu deren Spezifikation, Schätzung und Beurteilung. Zeitschrift für betriebswirtschaftliche Forschung 58:34–66

    Google Scholar 

  • Homburg C, Hildebrandt L (1998) Die Kausalanalyse: Bestandsaufnahme, Entwicklungsrichtungen, Problemfelder. In: Hildebrandt L, Homburg C (eds) Die Kausalanalyse, Stuttgart, pp 15–44

  • Homburg C, Klarmann M (2006) Die Kausalanalyse in der empirischen betriebswirtschaftlichen Forschung—Problemfelder und Anwendungsempfehlungen. DBW Die—Betriebswirtschaft 66:727–748

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoyer WD, Ridgway NM (1984) Variety seeking as an explanation for exploratory purchase behavior: a theoretical model. J Consum Res 11:114–119

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoyle RH, Panter AT (1995) Writing about structural equation models. In: Hoyle RH (ed) Structural equation modeling: concepts, issues, and applications. Sage, Thousand Oaks, pp 158–176

  • Hu L-T, Bentler PM (1995) Evaluating model fit. In: Hoyle RH (ed) Structural equation modelling: concepts, issues, and applications. Thousand Oaks, pp 76–99

  • Huber F, Herrmann A (2006) Organisationale Erfolgsfaktoren von Markenallianzen—Eine kausalanalytische Studie auf Basis des Ressourcenansatzes. DBW—Die Betriebswirtschaft 66:86–108

    Google Scholar 

  • Joachimsthaler EA, Lastovicka JA (1984) Optimal stimulation level—exploratory behavior models. J Consum Res 11:830–835

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joreskog KG, Sorbom D (1993) LISREL 8: structural equation modeling with the SIMPLIS command language. Scientific Software International, Chicago

  • Kahn BE, Isen AE (1993) The influence of positive affect on variety seeking among safe, enjoyable products. J Consum Res 20:257–270

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahn BE, Lehmann DR (1991) Modeling choice among assortments. J Retail 67:274–299

    Google Scholar 

  • Kassarjian WM (1962) A study of Riesman’s theory of social character. Sociometry 25:213–230

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keller H, Voss H-G (1976) Neugier und Exploration—Theorien und Ergebnisse. Kohlhammer, Stuttgart

  • Koppelmann U, Brodersen K, Volkmann M (2001) Wissen—Produktmarketing—Variety Seeking: Manchmal reizt auch nur das Neue. Absatzwirtschaft, 2001, No. 12, pp. 56

  • Krampen G (1979) Differenzierung des Konstrukts der Kontrollüberzeugungen: Deutsche Bearbeitung und Anwendung der IPC-Skalen. Zeitschrift für experimentelle und angewandte Psychologie 26:573–595

    Google Scholar 

  • Laurent G, Kapferer J-N (1985) Measuring consumer involvement profiles. J Mark Res 22:41–53

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacCallum RC, Browne MW, Sugawara HM (1996) Power analysis and determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psychol Methods 1:130–149

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manning KC, Bearden WO, Madden TJ (1995) Consumer innovativeness and the adoption process. J Consum Psychol 4/4:329–345

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McAlister L, Pessemier E (1982) Variety-seeking behavior: an interdisciplinary review. J Consum Res 9:311–322

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McReynolds P (1971) The nature and assessment of intrinsic motivation. In: McReynolds P (ed) Advances in psychological assessment 2. Science and behaviour Books, Palo Alto, pp 157–177

  • Midgley DF, Dowling GR (1978) Innovativeness: the concept and its measurement. J Consum Res 4:229–242

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mittal B, Lee M-S (1989) A causal model of consumer involvement. J Econ Psychol 10:363–389

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mittelstaedt RA, Grossbart SL, Curtis WW, De Vere SP (1976) Optimal stimulation level and the adoption process. J Consum Res 3:84–94

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Netemeyer RG, Boles JS, McKee DO, McMurrian R (1997) An investigation into the antecedents of organizational citizenship behaviors in a personal selling context. J Market 61:85–98

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholson N, Soane E, Fenton-O’Creevy M, Willman P (2005) Personality and domain-specific risk taking. J Risk Res 8/2:157–176

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orth UR (2005) Consumer personality and other factors in situational brand choice variation. Brand Manag 13/2:115–133

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orth UR, Bourrain A (2005a) Optimum stimulation theory and the differential impact of olfactory stimuli on consumer exploratory tendencies. Adv Consum Res 32:613–619

    Google Scholar 

  • Orth UR, Bourrain A (2005b) Ambient scent and consumer exploratory behaviour: a causal analysis. J Wine Res 16/2:137–150

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Otis LP (1984) Factors influencing the willingness to taste unusual foods. Psychol Rep 54:739–745

    Google Scholar 

  • Pervin LA, Cervone D, John OP (2005) Personality—theory and research. 9th edn. Wiley, New Jersey

  • Raju PS (1980) Optimum stimulation level: its relationship to personality, demographics, and exploratory behavior. J Consum Res 7:272–282

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roehm HA, Roehm ML (2005) Revisiting the effect of positive mood on variety seeking. J Consum Res 32:330–335

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbloom T (2003) Risk evaluation and risky behavior of high and low sensation seekers. Social Behav Personal 31/4:375–386

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rotter JB (ed) (1966) Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. American Psychological Associations, Washington

  • Rudolph B (1998) Kundenzufriedenheit im Industriegüterbereich. Gabler, Wiesbaden

  • Scholderer J, Balderjahn I (2006) Was unterscheidet harte und weiche Strukturgleichungsmodelle nun wirklich? Mark ZFP 28/1:57–70

    Google Scholar 

  • Schreier M, Oberhauser S, Prügl R (2007) Lead users and the adoption and diffusion of new products: insights from two extreme sports communities. Mark Lett 18:15–30

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonson I (1990) The effect of purchase quantity and timing on variety seeking behavior. J Mark Res 27:150–162

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonson I, Winer RS (1992) The influence of purchase quantity and display format on consumer preference for variety. J Consum Res 19:133–138

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steenkamp J-BEM, Baumgartner H (1992) The role of optimum stimulation level in exploratory consumer behavior. J Consum Res 19:434–448

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steenkamp J-BEM, Baumgartner H (1995) Development and cross-cultural validation of a short form of csi as a measure of optimum stimulation level. Int J Res Mark 12:97–104

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steenkamp J-BEM, Burgees SM (2002) Optimum stimulation level and exploratory consumer behavior in an emerging consumer market. Int J Res Mark 19:131–150

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Synder CR, Fromkin HL (1980) Uniqueness: the human pursuit of difference. New York

  • Tauber EM (1972) Why do people shop? J Mark 36:46–59

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ter Haseborg F, Mäßen A (1997) Das Phänomen des Variety-Seeking-Behavior: Modellierung, empirische Befunde und marketingpolitische Implikationen. Jahrbuch der Absatz- und Verbraucherforschung, 1997, No 2, pp 164–188

  • van Trijp HCM (1994) Product-related determinants of variety-seeking behavior for foods. Appetite 22:1–10

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Trijp HCM, Steenkamp J-BEM (1992) Consumer’s variety seeking tendency with respect to foods: measurement and managerial implications. Eur Rev Agric Econ 19:181–195

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Trijp HCM, Hoyer WD, Inman JJ (1996) Why switch? Product-category explanations for true variety-seeking behavior. J Mark Res 33:281–292

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Venkatraman MP, McInnis DJ (1985) The epistemic and sensory explanatory behaviors of hedonic and cognitive consumers. Adv Consum Res 12:102–107

    Google Scholar 

  • Venkatraman MP, Price LL (1990) Differentiating between cognitive and sensory innovativeness. J Bus Res 20:293–315

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wahlers RG, Etzel MJ (1985) A consumer response to incongruity between optimal stimulation and life style satisfaction. Adv Consum Res 12:97–101

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wahlers RG, Dunn MG, Etzel MJ (1986) The congruence of alternative OSL measures with consumer exploratory behavior tendencies. Adv Consum Res 13:398–402

    Google Scholar 

  • Warshaw PR (1980) Predicting purchase and other behaviors from general and contextually specific intentions. J Mark Res 17:26–33

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zuckerman M (1991) One person’s stress is another person’s pleasure. Stress Emotion, Anxiety, Anger, Curiosity 14:31–45

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgement

The authors appreciate the helpful comments of Hans Baumgartner and two anonymous reviewers.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Roland Helm.

Appendices

Appendix 1

“OSL” was measured by a short 7-item form of the Change Seeker Scale (CSI) first developed by Garlington and Shimota (1964) and later optimized by Steenkamp and Baumgartner (1995).

osl_csi1

I rather like doing something new or different than always doing the same things.

osl_csi2

In everyday life, I like having change and making novel experiences.

osl_csi3

I prefer to lead a life that facilitates change, variety, and travel even if this means to be exposed to certain risks.

osl_csi4

I am always seeking for new ideas and experiences.

osl_csi5

It is appealing to always do something different.

osl_csi6

When things get boring, I am trying to make new experiences.

osl_csi7

I prefer a steady way of life compared to an unpredictable way of life with lot’s of change.

The deviation of “OSL” and “ASL” was measured by a 7-item DCSI-scale, measuring the subjective difference between OSL and ASL directly, thus avoiding the problems attributed to conventional differential scales. Testing the DCSI-scale in a preliminary study against conventional approaches to measure the deviation of OSL and ASL was in support of regarding DCSI as being the advanced scale (Helm 2001).

diff1

Recently, I do … the same things too often … too many new things

diff2

Recently, I experience … too much change … too less change

diff3

Recently, my life is … too exciting … too less exciting

diff4

Recently, I have … too many new ideas … too less new ideas

diff5

Recently, I do … always something different … always the same

diff6

Recently, I make … too many new experiences … too less new experiences

diff7

Recently, my life is … too steady … too unsteady

RT was measured by a reduced 9-item scale following Raju (1980) and Wahlers et al. (1986).

rt1

I rather wait until others have tried new things before I am trying myself.

rt2

I am among the first to try out new products or brands.

rt3

I like trying out unusual things even if I am not certain that it is worth giving it a try.

rt4

I stick to products/brands that I usually buy rather than trying out something else that I am not sure about.

rt5

I never buy anything that I do not feel sufficiently informed about.

rt6

When purchasing expensive equipment I only consider well-known brands.

rt7

Only as an exception I buy products/brands which I am not familiar with.

rt8

I am very cautious when trying out unknown products/brands.

rt9

I think it is safer to buy products/brands that I am already familiar with.

VS was also measured following the scale of Raju (1980) with some adjustments of some of the ten items.

vs1

I think it is boring to always buy the same brands even if they are alright.

vs2

In order to have some change, I consciously choose among the brands that I already know.

vs3

To have a little variety, I occasionally like to change between brands I know.

vs4

There are many products at whom I permanently switch between brands when shopping.

vs5

There are many products, where I always switch among a few brands.

vs6

If I buy another brand than the one I usually buy, I would only consider one I am already familiar with.

vs7

Before buying an unknown brand I rather try to find a familiar brand.

vs8

To not always buy the same brands, I shop among a few different brands that I am already familiar with.

vs9

Very often I feel urged to buy something that is very different to the brands I usually buy.

vs10

When I am satisfied with a brand, I rarely change to another brand just to try out something different.

CMB alike was measured following the scale of Raju (1980) with ten adjusted items.

cm1

It is interesting to inform oneself about many brands of a product category even if one is not planning to buy anything.

cm2

A new store or restaurant is not among the things I am keen on finding out more about.

cm3

It is not necessary to always buy something just to have some variety. Sometimes, it is already stimulating just to glance around while shopping.

cm4

For new impressions regarding things I routinely buy, I rather read commercials or I have a look at store displays than trying out another brand.

cm5

Sometimes I amble through stores with curiosity without planning to buy anything.

cm6

I like testing free product samples of different brands because it enables me to compare.

cm7

If I see a new brand that seems to be somewhat different to the one I usually buy, I am keen on finding out more about it.

cm8

I don’t like window-shopping.

cm9

I like exploring different alternatives or brands when shopping.

cm10

I often look through mail order catalogues even if I am not planning to order anything.

Uniqueness seeking was measured by a four-item short form of the “Need for Uniqueness Scale” introduced by Synder and Fromkin (1980).

e1

I sometimes like to disregard social norms.

e2

It bothers me, if people think I am conventional.

e3

If I feel myself being very distinguished from the others in a group, I don’t feel comfortable.

e4

I tend to defend my opinion emphatically even if it is an extraordinary one.

Cognitive orientation was measured by four items of the Cognitive Innovativeness Scale by Venkatraman and Price (1990).

kog1

I often try to analyse my own feelings and reactions.

kog2

If I am confronted by extraordinary statements, I often try to find out what lies behind it.

kog3

It is not unusual for me to ponder over the causes why the world is like it is.

kog4

I often try to explain things in different ways.

Social character was measured by five items of the I–O Social Preference Scale of Kassarjian (1962).

s1

It is important to me what my friends think about me.

s2

I don’t feel comfortable, if I am not dressed in the fashion of my friends.

s3

I like to be popular and esteemed by others.

s4

In regards of my actions it is less important to me which impression I leave behind then what is best for me.

s5

For making important decisions, the advice of my friends means a lot to me.

Locus of control was measured by five items according to Krampen (1979) and based on the Internal–External Scale of Rotter (1966).

k1

Having a car accident or not merely depends on my driving skills.

k2

If I make plans, I am sure that the plans are put into action.

k3

I can control much of what is happening in my life.

k4

If I get what I want, it is most likely the result of hard work.

k5

My life is determined by my own actions.

Product involvement was measured by two items following the approach of Mittal and Lee (1989) based on Laurent and Kapferer (1985). Like for the following other product-category variables, adjustments in regards of the respective product were made.

inv1

Aftershave/Perfume doesn’t matter to me.

inv2

The usage of aftershave/perfume is very important to me.

The perceived hedonic value was as well operationalised according to Mittal and Lee (1989) based on Laurent and Kapferer (1985).

hed1

For me it is very satisfying to buy aftershave/perfume.

hed2

It is a real pleasure for me to use aftershave/perfume.

The degree of preferences was measured following the approach of van Trijp et al. (1996) by only one item.

praef

In the case of aftershave/perfume, I clearly prefer one or few brands relative to others.

Interpurchase frequency was also measured by one single item on a 7-point rating scale that was formulated sensitive to the conceivable possibilities within a respective product category.

Finally, product-category-specific exploratory behaviour as a tendency to choose either new or familiar alternatives against the formerly used alternative was measured following the approach of Raju (1980) as demonstrated in this example of aftershave/perfume:

Imagine you intend to buy an aftershave/a perfume. At the store you have the choice between the following brands:

Brand A

is the brand you have recently bought most often and which you have been completely satisfied with.

Brand B

is a band you are familiar with and that you believe to be equivalent to A.

Brand C

is a whole new brand to the market.

Now assume, brand B and C cost just as much as brand A. Would you be willing to buy brand B or C instead of brand A?

Willingness to buy brand B

very low … very high

Willingness to buy brand C

very low … very high

Imagine now, you would definitely like to try out something else. Do you …

definitely buy brand B … definitely buy brand C

Last question was aimed on validating the measurement of the willingness to take risk of a test person by his/her choice of either a new or a familiar product.

Appendix 2

Factor-analytical optimization and validation of the variables of intrinsic motivation and general exploratory tendencies.

Variable

Item

Factor loadings

Eigen values

Explained variance (%)

Cronbach α

OSL by CSI scale

osl_csi1r

0.612

3.313

47.33

0.806

osl_csi2

0.716

osl_csi3

0.733

osl_csi4

0.717

osl_csi5

0.762

osl_csi6

0.655

osl_csi7r

0.605

Subjective deviation of OSL and ASL by DCSI scale

diff1r

0.771

3.802

54.32

0.854

diff2

0.813

diff3

0.668

diff4

0.453

diff5

0.806

diff6

0.774

diff7r

0.805

Forming the three variables of exploratory consumer behaviour individually using principal component analysis provided the following results:

Variable

Item

Factor loadings

Eigen values

Explained variance (%)

Cronbach α

Risk taking

rt1

0.569

3.131

52.18

0.808

rt4

0.772

rt5

0.635

rt7

0.782

rt8

0.804

rt9

0.742

Variety seeking

vs3

0.675

2.243

56.07

0.737

vs4

0.775

vs5

0.779

vs8

0.762

Curiosity-motivated behaviour

cm1

0.602

2.644

44.07

0.731

cm3

0.477

cm5

0.819

cm8r

0.777

cm9

0.668

cm10

0.577

Examining the stability of the variables using a simultaneous principal axis analysis provided the following results:

Item

Factor loadings

Factor 1: risk taking (Eigen value: 3.474)

Factor 2: variety seeking (Eigen value: 2.971)

Factor 3: curiosity-motivated behaviour (Eigen value: 1.730)

rt1

0.558

−0.060

−0.033

rt4

0.773

−0.034

0.025

rt5

0.643

0.032

0.061

rt7

0.770

0.035

−0.052

rt8

0.798

0.077

−0.050

rt9

0.724

−0.048

0.095

vs3

−0.111

−0.674

0.100

vs4

0.013

−0.791

−0.073

vs5

0.115

−0.746

0.047

vs8

0.112

−0.750

−0.058

cm1

0.029

0.018

0.590

cm3

0.191

0.075

0.545

cm5

−0.047

−0.029

0.806

cm8r

−0.159

−0.031

0.738

cm9

−0.247

−0.183

0.576

cm10

0.028

0.018

0.590

Explained variance: 51.09%; KMO (matrix): 0.810

Appendix 3

Within-convergent-validity and discriminant-validity was successfully assessed for all exogenous variables. Nevertheless, the following tables are only concerned with the three forms of general exploratory tendencies under discussion in order to illustrate the fact that RT, VS, and CMB are distinct variables.

Within-convergent-validation of the different forms of general exploratory tendencies and OSL:

 

Model fit indices

Identified value

Threshold value

 

Overall fit statistics

χ2/df

2.221

<3

 

GFI

0.946

>0.9

 

AGFI

0.928

>0.9

 

SRMR

0.049

<0.05

 

RMSEA

0.032

<0.05

 
 

Item

Loading (>0.4)

t-value (>1.645)

Item reliability (>0.4)

Construct reliability (>0.6)

Extracted variance (>0.5)

Risk taking

rt1

0.460

9.604

0.212

0.929

0.694

rt4

0.716

16.362

0.519

rt5

0.380

7.409

0.147

rt7

0.664

14.532

0.441

rt8

0.701

15.553

0.492

rt9

0.650

14.369

0.428

Variety seeking

vs3

0.476

9.145

0.226

0.831

0.556

vs4

0.652

12.888

0.427

vs5

0.662

13.016

0.428

vs8

0.618

12.136

0.382

Curiosity-motivated behaviour

cm1

0.464

7.680

0.214

0.864

0.531

cm3

0.323

6.018

0.104

cm5

0.649

10.355

0.422

cm8r

0.602

10.250

0.363

cm9

0.631

10.740

0.400

cm10

0.422

7.979

0.179

Assessment of discriminant-validity by χ2-difference tests shows significant declines in the model fit when fixing correlations between any of the constructs to 1. Thus, disriminant-validity is supported.

 

Risk taking

Variety seeking

Variety seeking

277.55

 

Curiosity-motivated behaviour

353.20

27.12

Discriminant-validity is also supported by the Fornell–Larcker-Criterion. All squared correlations between the constructs (0.022; 0.053; 0.050) are smaller than the respective extracted variances.

 

Extracted variance

Variety seeking

Curiosity-motivated behaviour

Risk taking

0.694

0.022

0.053

Variety seeking

0.556

 

0.050

Curiosity-motivated behaviour

0.531

  

Appendix 4

Factor-analytical optimization and validation of the personality trait variables by principal component analysis.

Variable

Item

Factor loadings

Eigen values

Explained variance (%)

Cronbach α

Uniqueness seeking

e1

0.739

1.461

48.71

0.476

e3r

0.667

e4

0.685

Cognitive orientation

kog1

0.733

2.310

57.74

0.740

kog2

0.813

kog3

0.711

kog4

0.779

Social character

s1

0.830

1.752

58.39

0.640

s3

0.748

s5

0.709

Locus of control

k2

0.637

1.875

46.87

0.611

k3

0.783

k4

0.630

k5

0.678

For three of the variable scales the reliability was not satisfying. A simultaneous principal axis analysis was conducted in order to test, if it is still possible to model the four assumed variables.

Examining the stability of the personality trait variables using a simultaneous principal axis analysis provided the following results:

Item

Factor loadings

Factor 1: uniqueness seeking (Eigen value 2.886)

Factor 2: locus of control (Eigen value 1.962)

Factor 3: social character (Eigen value 1.607)

Factor 4: cogn. orientation (Eigen value 1.147)

e1

0.583

−0.049

0.065

0.010

e3r

0.422

0.042

−0.202

−0.053

e4

0.418

0.067

0.016

0.120

k2

−0.080

0.485

−0.062

0.065

k3

0.050

0.723

0.094

−0.059

k4

−0.064

0.461

−0.026

−0.038

k5

0.116

0.469

0.094

0.071

s1

−0.122

−0.014

0.767

−0.013

s3

0.025

0.087

0.593

−0.032

s5

0.027

−0.043

0.491

0.047

kog1

−0.062

−0.018

0.082

0.623

kog2

0.107

0.037

0.081

0.696

kog3

−0.081

−0.072

−0.041

0.597

kog4

0.045

0.025

−0.094

0.696

Explained variance: 54.30%; KMO (matrix): 0.705

All Eigen values were above 1 and all items can be clearly allocated to a single variable without violating any criteria. Correlations between the variables are low. Altogether, the measurements of personality traits are applicable for further analysis.

In regards of product-category specific variables, however, high correlations between the variables led to an extraction of only one factor variable in all of the scenarios. Therefore, only involvement and the degree of preferences went into further analysis.

Appendix 5

Factor loadings of the exogenous models (in the six different product settings):

Factor

Item

Cell phone

Perfume

Hairdresser

Alternatives of frozen food

Alternatives of pizza brands

Alternatives of pizza toppings

OSL

osl_csi1r

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

osl_csi2

0.588

0.585

0.586

0.591

0.588

0.589

osl_csi3

0.585

0.582

0.583

0.581

0.584

0.583

osl_csi4

0.587

0.584

0.585

0.587

0.589

0.589

osl_csi5

0.619

0.614

0.616

0.618

0.616

0.615

osl_csi6

0.447

0.444

0.445

0.445

0.445

0.445

osl_csi7r

0.510

0.510

0.511

0.513

0.511

0.513

OSL-ASL

diff1r

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

diff2

0.650

0.652

0.653

0.653

0.652

0.652

diff3

0.645

0.645

0.645

0.643

0.643

0.643

diff4

0.522

0.523

0.525

0.520

0.522

0.521

diff5

0.634

0.632

0.633

0.633

0.630

0.629

diff6

0.437

0.434

0.437

0.436

0.434

0.435

diff7r

0.507

0.506

0.506

0.500

0.501

0.502

Uniqueness seeking

e1

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

e3r

0.313

0.308

0.314

0.296

0.297

0.297

e4

0.280

0.276

0.277

0.291

0.285

0.292

Locus of control

k2

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

k3

0.482

0.481

0.478

0.478

0.478

0.478

k4

0.320

0.319

0.317

0.319

0.317

0.317

k5

0.365

0.365

0.366

0.367

0.364

0.364

Social character

s1

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

s3

0.438

0.444

0.441

0.444

0.442

0.441

s5

0.432

0.431

0.426

0.431

0.430

0.435

Cogn. orientation

kog1

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

kog2

0.705

0.701

0.706

0.713

0.713

0.712

kog3

0.485

0.478

0.483

0.483

0.482

0.480

kog4

0.599

0.593

0.596

0.597

0.595

0.599

Product involvement

inv1

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

inv2r

0.672

0.675

0.366

0.516

0.520

0.519

Degree of preferences

präf

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

Appendix 6

Factor loadings of the endogenous models (in the six different product setting):

Factor

Item

Cell phone

Perfume

Hairdresser

Alternatives of frozen food

Alternatives of pizza brands

Alternatives of pizza toppings

Risk taking

rt1

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

rt4

0.745

0.751

0.744

0.751

0.752

0.754

rt5

0.437

0.444

0.444

0.449

0.450

0.449

rt7

0.621

0.619

0.614

0.619

0.614

0.612

rt8

0.629

0.633

0.631

0.642

0.645

0.646

rt9

0.550

0.553

0.550

0.546

0.546

0.547

Variety seeking

vs3

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

vs4

0.616

0.611

0.630

0.633

0.616

0.626

vs5

0.638

0.631

0.639

0.618

0.612

0.626

vs8

0.524

0.531

0.526

0.518

0.531

0.510

Curiosity-motivated behaviour

cm1

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

1.000

cm3

0.200

0.202

0.202

0.197

0.199

0.200

cm5

0.633

0.643

0.639

0.634

0.636

0.637

cm8r

0.653

0.667

0.660

0.658

0.662

0.658

cm9

0.719

0.714

0.712

0.715

0.716

0.714

cm10

0.405

0.404

0.400

0.404

0.405

0.405

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Helm, R., Landschulze, S. Optimal stimulation level theory, exploratory consumer behaviour and product adoption: an analysis of underlying structures across product categories. Rev Manag Sci 3, 41–73 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11846-009-0024-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11846-009-0024-7

Keywords

  • Optimal stimulation theory
  • Consumer behaviour
  • Exploratory consumer behaviour
  • Adoption
  • New products

JEL Classification

  • M31