Skip to main content
Log in

Believing is seeing: an fMRI study of thought-action fusion in healthy male adults

  • Original Research
  • Published:
Brain Imaging and Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Thought-action fusion (TAF) is a tendency of individuals to establish causal relations between their own thoughts and external reality. TAF can lead to maladaptive behaviors typically observed in obsessional thoughts. However, neural mechanisms underlying TAF are still unknown. In this study, 38 healthy men were informed that MR signals were able to detect thoughts of the word ‘apple’ and that this recognition could result in the administration of electrical shocks to a person outside the scanner. During MR acquisition, they were asked to suppress or not suppress the thought of ‘apple’ while sham electrical shocks were or were not administered to the other person. The main effect of the sham administration of electrical shock to another person was shown in the bilateral lingual gyri, fusiform gyri, and middle occipital cortices (FDR corrected p < 0.05). Also, fusiform gyrus, lingual gyrus, and middle occipital cortex activity correlated with scores of guilty feeling only when participants consciously tried to think of apple as less as possible. Our study demonstrates that visual association areas may play primary roles in TAF. The simple belief and visual imagery that one’s thought may lead to someone’s injury activated visual areas of the brain where, in turn, brain activity is associated with feelings of guilt.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abramowitz, J. S., Whiteside, S., Lynam, D., & Kalsy, S. (2003). Is thought-action fusion specific to obsessive-compulsive disorder? A mediating role of negative affect. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 1069–1079.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berle, D., & Starcevic, V. (2005). Thought-action fusion: Review of the literature and future directions. Clinical Psychology Review, 25(3), 263–284.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berntson, G. G., Norman, G. J., Bechara, A., Bruss, J., Tranel, D., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2011). The insula and evaluative processes. Psychological Science, 22(1), 80–86.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bocci, L., & Gordon, P. K. (2007). Does magical thinking produce neutralising behaviour? An experimental investigation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 1823–1833.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M. M., Sabatinelli, D., Lang, P. J., Fitzsimmons, J. R., King, W., & Desai, P. (2003). Activation of the visual cortex in motivated attention. Behavioral Neuroscience, 117(2), 369–380.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bub, K., & Lommen, M. (2017). The role of guilt in posttraumatic stress disorder. European Journal of Pychotraumatology, 8(1), 1407202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Candea, D. M., & Szentagotai-Tata, A. (2018). Shame-proneness, guilt-proneness and anxiety symptoms: A meta-analysis. Journal of Anxiety Disorder, 58, 78–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D. M. (1999). Anxiety disorders: Why they persist and how to treat them. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 5–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Gelder, B., Tamietto, M., Pegna, A. J., & Van den Stock, J. (2015). Visual imagery influences brain responses to visual stimulation in bilateral cortical blindness. Cortex, 72, 15–26.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fergus, T. A., Valentiner, D. P., McGrath, P. B., & Jencius, S. (2010). Shame- and guilt-proneness: Relationships with anxiety disorder symptoms in a clinical sample. Journal of Anxiety Disorder, 24(8), 811–815.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fulford, J., Milton, F., Salas, D., Smith, A., Simler, A., Winlove, C., & Zeman, A. (2018). The neural correlates of visual imagery vividness - an fMRI study and literature review. Cortex, 105, 26–40.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Garfinkel, S. N., & Critchley, H. D. (2013). Interoception, emotion and brain: new insights link internal physiology to social behaviour. Commentary on:: "Anterior insular cortex mediates bodily sensibility and social anxiety" by Terasawa et al. (2012). Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(3), 231–234.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Guillot, A., Collet, C., Nguyen, V. A., Malouin, F., Richards, C., & Doyon, J. (2009). Brain activity during visual versus kinesthetic imagery: An fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 30(7), 2157–2172.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hazlett-Stevens, H., Zucker, B. G., & Craske, M. G. (2002). The relationship of thought-action fusion to pathologicial worry and generalized anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40(10), 1199–1204.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hossein, G. K., Ne'mat, M., & Niloofar, M. (2012). Comparison of thought–action fusion in peo-ples with obsessive–compulsive disorder and major depression disorder. International Journal of Applied Psychology, 2, 77–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, R., & Bhattacharya, J. (2014). A role for the precuneus in thought-action fusion: Evidence from participants with significant obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Neuroimage-Clinical, 4, 112–121.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jarrett, R. B., & Weissenburger, J. E. (1990). Guilt in depressed outpatients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 58(4), 495–498.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, W. H., Schratter, A. K., & Kugler, K. (2000). The guilt inventory. Psychological Reports, 87(3), 1039–1042.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kleckner, I. R., Zhang, J., Touroutoglou, A., Chanes, L., Xia, C., Simmons, W. K., Quigley, K. S., Dickerson, B. C., & Barrett, L. F. (2017). Evidence for a large-scale brain system supporting Allostasis and Interoception in humans. Nature Human Behaviour, 1.

  • Knauff, M., Kassubek, J., Mulack, T., & Greenlee, M. W. (2000). Cortical activation evoked by visual mental imagery as measured by fMRI. Neuroreport, 11(18), 3957–3962.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kubany, E. S., & Manke, F. P. (1995). Cognitive therapy for trauma-related guilt: Conceptial bases and treatment outline. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 2(1), 27–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuniecki, M., Woloszyn, K., Domagalik, A., & Pilarczyk, J. (2018). Disentangling brain activity related to the processing of emotional visual information and emotional arousal. Brain Structure and Function, 223(4), 1589–1597.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ladouceur, R., Rheaume, J., Freeston, M. H., Aublet, F., Jean, K., Lachance, S., Langlois, F., & de Pokomandy-Morin, K. (1995). Experimental manipulations of responsibility: An analogue test for models of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(8), 937–946.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., Fitzsimmons, J. R., Cuthbert, B. N., Scott, J. D., Moulder, B., & Nangia, V. (1998). Emotional arousal and activation of the visual cortex: An fMRI analysis. Psychophysiology, 35(2), 199–210.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, H. J., Cougle, J. R., & Telch, M. J. (2005). Thought-action fusion and its relationship to schizotypy and OCD symptoms. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(1), 29–41.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, S. (2000). The relationships of OC symptoms with moral and causal responsibility and with omission (master). Seoul National University.

  • Lee, S. W., Cha, H., Chung, Y., Kim, E., Song, H., Chang, Y., & Lee, S. J. (2019). The neural correlates of thought-action fusion in healthy adults: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Depression and Anxiety, 36(8), 732–743.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mancini, F., & Gangemi, A. (2004). Fear of guilt from behaving irresponsibly in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 35(2), 109–120.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marcks, B. A., & Woods, D. W. (2007). Role of thought0related beliefs and coping strategies in the escalation of intrusive thoughts: An analog to obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 2640–2651.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Melli, G., Carraresi, C., Poli, A., Marazziti, D., & Pinto, A. (2017). The role of guilt sensitivity in OCD symptom dimensions. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 24(5), 1079–1089.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Michl, P., Meindl, T., Meister, F., Born, C., Engel, R. R., Reiser, M., & Hennig-Fast, K. (2014). Neurobiological underpinnings of shame and guilt: A pilot fMRI study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(2), 150–157.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Myers, S. G., Fisher, P. L., & Wells, A. (2008). Belief domains of the obsessive beliefs Questionnaire-44 (OBQ-44) and their specific relationship with obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22(3), 475–484.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pearson, J., Naselaris, T., Holmes, E. A., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2015). Mental imagery: Functional mechanisms and clinical applications. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(10), 590–602.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Pilarczyk, J., & Kuniecki, M. (2014). Emotional content of an image attracts attention more than visually salient features in various signal-to-noise ratio conditions. Journal of Vision, 14(12).

  • Pourtois, G., Schettino, A., & Vuilleumier, P. (2013). Brain mechanisms for emotional influences on perception and attention: What is magic and what is not. Biological Psychology, 92(3), 492–512.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rachman, S. (1993). Obsessions, responsibility and guilt. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31(2), 149–154.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rachman, S., & Shafran, R. (1999). Cognitive distortions: Thought-action fusion. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 6(2), 80–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rachman, S., Shafran, R., Mitchell, D., Trant, J., & Teachman, B. (1996). How to remain neutral: An experimental analysis of neutralization. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34(11–12), 889–898.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rassin, E., Merckelbach, H., Muris, P., & Spaan, V. (1999). Thought-action fusion as a causal factor in the development of intrusions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37(3), 231–237.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sabatinelli, D., Fortune, E. E., Li, Q., Siddiqui, A., Krafft, C., Oliver, W. T., Beck, S., & Jeffries, J. (2011). Emotional perception: Meta-analyses of face and natural scene processing. Neuroimage, 54(3), 2524–2533.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salkovskis, P. M. (1985). Obsessional-compulsive problems: A cognitive-behavioural analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23(5), 571–583.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salkovskis, P. M. (1989). Cognitive-behavioural factors and the persistence of intrusive thoughts in obsessional problems. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 27(6), 677–682.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shafran, R., & Rachman, S. (2004). Thought-action fusion: A review. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 35(2), 87–107.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shafran, R., Thordarson, D. S., & Rachman, S. (1996a). Thought-action fusion in obsessive compulsive disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 10(5), 379–391.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shafran, R., Watkins, E., & Charman, T. (1996b). Guilt in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 10(6), 509–516.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smari, J., & Holmsteinsson, H. E. (2001). Intrusive thoughts, responsibility attitudes, thought-action fusion, and chronic thought suppression in relation to obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 29(1), 13–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Takahashi, H., Yahata, N., Koeda, M., Matsuda, T., Asai, K., & Okubo, Y. (2004). Brain activation associated with evaluative processes of guilt and embarrassment: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 23(3), 967–974.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, U., N'Diaye, K., Ethofer, T., & Vuilleumier, P. (2011). Guilt-specific processing in the prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 21(11), 2461–2470.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., & White, T. L. (1987). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 5–13.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Winlove, C. I. P., Milton, F., Ranson, J., Fulford, J., MacKisack, M., Macpherson, F., & Zeman, A. (2018). The neural correlates of visual imagery: A co-ordinate-based meta-analysis. Cortex, 105, 4–25.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wyland, C. L., Kelley, W. M., Macrae, C. N., Gordon, H. L., & Heatherton, T. F. (2003). Neural correlates of thought suppression. Neuropsychologia, 41(14), 1863–1867.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This work was supported by the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) grant funded by the Korea government (MSIP) (NRF-2018R1A2B6007374).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Yongmin Chang or Seung Jae Lee.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

The present study was performed in agreement with the Declaration of Helsinki and its further amendments. The study is approved by the Institutional Review Board of Kyungpook National University Hospital (2018–04-029).

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

ESM 1

(DOCX 2829 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lee, S.W., Kim, E., Chung, Y. et al. Believing is seeing: an fMRI study of thought-action fusion in healthy male adults. Brain Imaging and Behavior 15, 300–310 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-020-00257-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-020-00257-y

Keywords

Navigation