Skip to main content
Log in

Control beliefs are associated with preserved memory function in the face of low hippocampal volume among diverse older adults

  • Original Research
  • Published:
Brain Imaging and Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Growing evidence supports a link between locus of control and memory in older adults. Control beliefs may directly predict larger hippocampal volume (i.e., brain maintenance). Alternatively, control beliefs may be associated with smaller hippocampal volume at any given level of memory (i.e., cognitive reserve). Multivariable regression analyses examined associations between verbal episodic memory, locus of control and hippocampal volume among 236 non-demented older adults in the community-based Washington Heights-Inwood Columbia Aging Project. Control beliefs were negatively associated with hippocampal volume when controlling for memory performance, indicating that individuals with stronger control beliefs were able to maintain memory function in the face of lower hippocampal volume. Subsequent exploratory models stratified by race/ethnicity indicated that this association was more prominent among racial/ethnic minorities (particularly Caribbean Hispanic older adults) than among non-Hispanic White older adults. Control beliefs were not associated with hippocampal volume before memory was taken into account. Results are consistent with the view that control beliefs facilitate the maintenance of memory function despite hippocampal volume loss. Results are not consistent with the view that locus of control contributes to brain maintenance. Culturally-appropriate interventions are needed to test whether strengthening control beliefs provides cognitive resistance to neuropathology.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Agrigoroaei, S., Neupert, S. D., & Lachman, M. E. (2013). Maintaining a sense of control in the context of cognitive challenge: greater stability in control beliefs benefits working memory. GeroPsychology, 26, 45–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Apostolova, L. G., Zarow, C., Biado, K., hurtz, S., Boccardi, M., et al. (2015). Relationship between hippocampal atrophy and neuropathology markers: a 7T MRI validation study of the EADC-ADNI harmonized hippocampal segmentation protocol. Alzheiemer’s & Dementia, 11, 139–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barulli, D., & Stern, Y. (2013). Efficiency, capacity, compensation, maintenance, plasticity: emerging concepts in cognitive reserve. Trends in Cognitive Science, 17, 502–509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brickman, A. M., Siedlecki, K. L., Muraskin, J., Manly, J. J., Luchsinger, J. A., Yeung, L.-K., Brown, T. R., DeCarli, C., & Stern, Y. (2011). White matter hyperintensities and cognition: testing the reserve hypothesis. Neurobiology of Aging, 32, 1588–1598.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bruce, M. A., & Thornton, M. C. (2004). It’s my world? Exploring black and white perceptions of personal control. The Sociological Quarterly, 45, 597–612.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buschke, H., & Fuld, P. A. (1974). Evaluating storage, retention, and retrieval in disordered memory and learning. Neurology, 24, 1019–1025.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • de Frias, C. M., Dixon, R. A., & Bäckman, L. (2003). Use of memory compensation strategies is related to psychosocial and health indicators. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 58B, P12-P22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Den Heijer, T., Geerlings, M. I., Hoebeek, F. E., Hofman, A., Koudstaal, P. J., & Breteler, M. M. (2006). Use of hippocampal and amygdalar volumes on magnetic resonance imaging to predict dementia in cognitively intact elderly people. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63, 57–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Den Heijer, T., van der Lijn, F., Koudstaal, P. J., Hofman, A., van der Lugt, A., et al. (2010). A 10-year follow-up of hippocampal volume on magnetic resonance imaging in early dementia and cognitive decline. Brain, 133, 1163–1172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forstmeier, S., & Maercker, A. (2008). Motivational reserve: lifetime motivational abilities contribute to cognitive and emotional health in old age. Psychology and Aging, 23, 886–899.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gorbach, T., Pudas, S., Lundquist, A., Orädd, G., Josefsson, M., Salami, A., de Luna, X., & Nyberg, L. (2016). Longitudinal association between hippocampus atrophy and episodic-memory decline. Neurobiology of Aging, 51, 167–176.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hertzog, C., McGuire, C. L., & Lineweaver, T. T. (1998). Aging, attributions, perceived control, and strategy use in a free recall task. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 5, 85–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hooker, K., & McAdams, D. P. (2003). Personality reconsidered: a new agenda for aging research. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 58B, P296-P304.

    Google Scholar 

  • Irwin, M., Haydari, K., & Oxman, M. N. (1999). Screening for depression in the older adult: criterion validity of the 10-item Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D). Archives of internal medicine, 159, 1701–1704.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, J., Miller, J., Lee, S. A., Coffey, T., Watrous, A. J., et al. (2016). Direct electrical stimulation of the human entorhinal region and hippocampus impairs memory. Neuron, 92, 983–990.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kay, A. C., Gaucher, D., Napier, J. L., Callin, M. J., & Laurin, K. (2008). God and the government: testing a compensatory control mechanism for the support of external systems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 18–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lachman, M. E., & Weaver, S. L. (1998). The sense of control as a moderator of social class differences in health and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 763–773.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lachman, M. E., & Agrigoroaei, S. (2012). Low perceived control as a risk factor for episodic memory: the mediational role of anxiety and task interference. Memory and Cognition, 40, 287–296.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lachman, M. E., Weaver, S. L., Bandura, M., Elliott, E., & Lewkowicz, C. J. (1992). Improving memory and control beliefs through cognitive restructuring and self-generated strategies. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 47B, P293-P299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lachman, M. E., Neupert, S. D., & Agrigoroaei, S. (2011). The relevance of control beliefs for health and aging. In K. W. Schaie & S. L. Willis (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (pp. 175–190). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Langer, E., & Rodin, J. (1996). Long-term effects of a control-relevant intervention with the institutionalized aged. In S. Fein & S. Spencer (Eds.), Reading in social psychology: The art and science of research (pp. 175–180). Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laurin, K., Kay, A. C., & Moscovitch, D. A. (2008). On the belief in God: Toward an understanding of the emotional substrates of compensatory control. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1559–1562.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manly, J. J., Bell-McGinty, S., Tang, M.-X., Schupf, N., Stern, Y., & Mayeux, R. (2005). Implementing diagnostic criteria and estimating frequency of mild cognitive impairment in an urban community. Archives of Neurology, 62, 1739–1746.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Melrose, R. J., Brewster, P., Marquine, M. J., MacKay-Brandt, A., et al. (2015). Early life development in a multiethnic sample and the relation to late life cognition. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 70, 519–531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, L. M. S., & Gagne, D. D. (2005). Effects of age and control beliefs on resource allocation during reading. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 12, 129–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neupert, S. D., & Allaire, J. C. (2012). I think I can, I think I can: examining the within-person coupling of control beliefs and cognition in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 27, 742–749.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nyberg, L., et al. (2012). Memory aging and brain maintenance. Trends in Cognitive Science, 16, 292–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pruessner, J. C., Baldwin, M. W., Dedovic, K., Renwick, R., Khalili Mahani, N., Lord, C., Meaney, M., & Lupien, S. (2005). Self-esteem, locus of control, hippocampal volume, and cortisol regulation in young and old adulthood. NeuroImage, 28, 815–826.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, C. E., & Mirowsky, J. (2013). The sense of personal control: social structural causes and emotional consequences. In C. S. Aneshensel, J. C. Phelan & A. Bierman (Eds.), Handbook of the Sociology of Mental Health (2nd edn.). Dordrecht: Springer.

  • Searle, M. S., Mahon, M. J., Iso-Ahola, S. E., Sdrolias, H. A., & van Dyck, J. (1998). Examining the long term effects of leisure education on a sense of independence and psychological well-being among the elderly. Journal of Leisure Research, 30, 331–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seeman, T., McAvay, G., Merrill, S., Albert, M., & Rodin, J. (1996). Self-efficacy beliefs and change in cognitive performance: MacArthur Studies of Successful Aging. Psychology and Aging, 11, 538–551. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.11.3.538.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Serra, L., Musicco, M., Cercignani, M., Torso, M., Spanò, B., Mastropasqua, C., et al. (2015). Cognitive reserve and the risk for Alzheimer’s disease: a longitudinal study. Neurobiology of Aging, 36, 592–600.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, B. A., & Krause, N. (2001). Exploring race variations in aging and personal control. Journals of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 56B, S119-S124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skaff, M. M., Mullan, J. T., Fisher, L., & Chesla, C. A. (2003). A contextual model of control beliefs, behavior, and health: Latino and European Americans with type 2 diabetes. Psychology & Health, 18, 295–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Squire, L. R., & Wixted, J. T. (2011). The cognitive neuroscience of human memory since H.M. Annual Reviews of Neuroscience, 34, 259–288.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, Y., Alexander, G. E., Prohovnik, I., & Mayeux, R. (1992). Inverse relationship between education and parietotemporal perfusion deficit in Alzheimer’s disease. Annals of Neurology, 32, 371–375.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tang, M.-X., Cross, P., Andrews, H., Jacobs, D. M., Small, S., Bell, K., Merchant, C., Lantigua, R., Costa, R., Stern, Y., & Mayeux, R. (2001). Incidence of AD in African-Americans, Caribbean Hispanics, and Caucasians in northern Manhattan. Neurology, 56, 49–56.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tennstedt, S., Howland, J., Lachman, M. E., Peterson, E. W., Kasten, L., & Jette, A. (1998). A randomized, controlled trial of a group intervention to reduce fear of falling and associated activity restriction in older adults. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 53B, P384-P392.

    Google Scholar 

  • U.S. Census Bureau. (2004). U.S. interim projections by age, sex, race, and Hispanic origin. Retrieved October 5, 2016, http://www.census.gov/ipc/www/usinterimproj/.

  • Van Petten, C. (2004). Relationship between hippocampal volume and memory ability in healthy individuals across the lifespan: review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1394–1413.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whitson, J. A., & Galinksky, A. D. (2008). Lacking control increases illusory pattern perception. Science, 322, 115–117.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, R. S., Capuano, A. W., Sytsma, J., Bennett, D. A., & Barnes, L. L. (2015). Cognitive aging in older Black and White persons. Psychology and Aging, 30, 279–285.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zahodne, L. B., Nowinski, C., Gershon, R., & Manly, J. J. (2014). Which psychosocial factors best predict cognitive performance in older adults? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 20(5), 487–495.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zahodne, L. B., Manly, J. J., Narkhede, A., Griffith, E. Y., DeCarli, C., et al. (2015a). Structural MRI predictors of late-life cognition differ across African Americans,Hispanics, and Whites. Current Alzheimer Research, 12, 632–639.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zahodne, L. B., Meyer, O. L., Choi, E., Thomas, M. L., Willis, S. L., Marsiske, M., Gross, A. L., Rebok, G. W., & Parisi, J. M. (2015b). External locus of control contributes to racial disparities in memory and reasoning training gains in ACTIVE. Psychology & Aging, 30(3), 561–572.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zahodne, L. B., Nowinski, C. J., Gershon, R. C., & Manly, J. J. (2015c). Self-efficacy buffers the relationship between educational disadvantage and executive functioning. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 21, 297–304.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Laura B. Zahodne.

Ethics declarations

Funding

Data collection and sharing for this project was supported by the Washington Heights-Inwood Columbia Aging Project (WHICAP, PO1AG07232, R01AG037212, RF1AG054023, R00AG047963, R01AG054520, R01AG034189, R56AG034189, R01AG016206) funded by the National Institute on Aging (NIA). This manuscript has been reviewed by WHICAP investigators for scientific content and consistency of data interpretation with previous WHICAP Study publications. We acknowledge the WHICAP study participants and the WHICAP research and support staff for their contributions to this study. This publication was supported by the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences, National Institutes of Health, through Grant Number UL1TR001873. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH.

Conflict of interest

Drs. Zahodne, Schupf, and Brickman declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures followed were in accordance with the ethical standards of the responsible committee on human experimentation (institutional and national) and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, and the applicable revisions at the time of the investigation.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all participants for being included in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Zahodne, L.B., Schupf, N. & Brickman, A.M. Control beliefs are associated with preserved memory function in the face of low hippocampal volume among diverse older adults. Brain Imaging and Behavior 12, 1112–1120 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-017-9776-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-017-9776-x

Keywords

Navigation