Skip to main content
Log in

Wirkung gendersensibler Ansprachen in Anmoderationen bei Erwachsenen und Heranwachsenden

Effects of gender-fair addressing for audiovisual journalistic media among adults and adolescents

  • Aufsatz
  • Published:
Publizistik Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Zusammenfassung

Die aktuelle Debatte über den Einsatz geschlechtergerechter Sprache in (Medien‑)Texten aufgreifend, untersucht dieser Beitrag die Wahrnehmung und Wirkung von gendersensiblen Ansprachen in audiovisuellen journalistischen Medienumgebungen. Der Aufsatz greift dabei auf Befunde aus der Kommunikationswissenschaft und Genderlinguistik zur Wirkung von gendersensibler Sprache im Hinblick auf Verständlichkeit und wahrgenommene Geschlechterrepräsentation zurück. Er erweitert diesen Forschungstand um die Analyse gesprochener Sprache im Journalismus. Zudem betrachtet die vorliegende Studie sowohl Erwachsene als auch Heranwachsende als Rezipierende. In empirischer Hinsicht werden zwei Online-Experimentalstudien vorgestellt, die jeweils auf audio-(visuelle) Stimuli bei der Anmoderation von Nachrichtenbeiträgen zurückgreifen. Die Manipulation erfolgte bei jeweils drei Formen gendersensibler Personenbezeichnungen, der Doppelnennung, der Gender_Gap-Sprechweise und der Neutralisierung, sowie beim generischen Maskulinum. Die Untersuchung schließt N = 770 Erwachsene und N = 142 Heranwachsende ein. Zentraler Befund beider Studien ist, dass einzig die Gender_Gap-Sprechweise als signifikant schlechter verständlich bewertet wird. Gleichzeitig führt diese Ansprache zu einem signifikant höher geschätzten Anteil an Frauen in den Beiträgen.

Abstract

This article contributes to the current debate on the use of gender-fair language. Gender has developed into a disputed political issue in Western democracies and the use of gender fair language is one issue of concern, with conservative politicians often advocating to ban its usage from public discourse. In this study, we focus on the use of gender-fair language in media texts, particularly in audiovisual news. The paper draws on findings from communication studies and gender linguistics on the effect of gender-fair language in terms of comprehensibility and perceived gender representation. A peculiarity of the German language (and other languages, such as French) in contrast to English is the use of the grammatical form of generic masculinity to denote the plural of groups of persons. This use of the generic masculine form has come under criticism from feminist scholars and gender linguistics arguing that it reduces the visibility of female and non-binary persons. Therefore, advocates of gender-fair language use argue for new forms of address that include women but also non-binary persons. Some alternatives, such as double naming e.g., addressing both male and female representatives, or neutralization, e.g., gender-unmarked forms, have already been investigated by research both in communication as well as gender linguistics. Newer forms include the so-called gender_gap (marked in written form with a _, * or :). Research has demonstrated that gender-fair forms can lead to a higher perceived representation of women without reducing comprehensibility, once people become accustomed to them.

Even though the state of research is solid with respect to written text, little research has been devoted to media text nor, to our understanding, to spoken language within journalistic formats. At the same time, we witness a growing sensitivity to the use of gender-fair language among TV and radio presenters, both in programming for adults and on public service children’s TV. Criticism against gender-fair language often comes from older persons and it appears that younger media users are more accepting towards (and perhaps accustomed to) gender-fair language. This has led public service broadcasters to open the way for gender-fair language in children’s news.

Therefore, in this study, we expand the current state of research by focusing on spoken language as part of short announcements in TV (or radio) news. In a second step, we interrogate if effects we find within the adult population relate to effects found among younger audiences. From an empirical point of view, we conducted two online experimental studies each of which used audio (visual) stimuli in the introduction to a TV news report. In each case, the script was manipulated to use either generic masculine formulations or one of three forms of gender-fair formulations. The following gender-fair formulations were used: Double naming, gender_gap (spoken with a pause between the male occupation title and its female ending), and neutralization. The stimuli were produced by real (male) news presenters from public service broadcasters. As stimulus material, we used announcements referring to population groups which varied in terms of their typical gender balance: (a) police (a greater proportion of males), nurses (a greater proportion of females) and schoolkids (an even gender distribution).

770 adults and 142 adolescents took part in the two online surveys. The results show that, in line with existing studies on written language, the use of gender-fair language only minimally impacts comprehensibility, at least in the short introductions studied. For two of the gender-fair formulations (double naming and neutralization), we did not find evidence of any significant reduction in comprehensibility compared with the generic masculine formulation, neither for adolescents nor adults. However, the gender_gap version was found to be significantly less comprehensible by both groups. Overall, the comprehensibility of all announcements was rather high, even for the children’s sample and patterns between children and adults did not differ substantially.

Meanwhile, with one exception (neutralization, nurses) neither neutralization nor double naming led to significantly higher estimations of the proportion of females within the following news pieces compared to the generic masculine form. Only the gender_gap formulation led to such higher estimations. Meanwhile, in all conditions, participants were sensitive to the actual gender distributions of each population group, i.e. they estimated higher proportions of men among the police, and higher proportions of women amongst nursed.

Discussing our findings, we argue effects of comprehensibility of gender-fair language are most likely due to habituation of word-usage. Children did not react negatively to forms of gender-fair language and anecdotal evidence from the adult sample shows that it is only the gender_gap version that resulted in emotionally charged negative reactions. Yet, at the same time differences between the generic masculine and gender_gap forms in terms of comprehensibility, albeit significant, are rather small.

One limitation of our studies is that we were only able to focus on the representation of male vs. female group members. We did not account for non-binary persons both in our announcements as well as in our samples. Future research is required to further open the ground for this issue. We encourage researchers within our field to further investigate effects of gender-fair language in (news) media.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Notes

  1. Per MANCOVA kontrollierten wir zusätzlich den Einfluss soziodemographischer Variablen sowie mögliche Interaktionen mit dem Geschlecht der Proband*innen. Diese hatten jedoch keinen Einfluss auf die Effekte der Ansprache.

  2. Auch hier kontrollierten wir den Einfluss soziodemographischer Variablen auf den Effekt der Ansprache mit MANCOVA. Die gefunden Effekte blieben bestehen.

Literatur

  • Abou-Chadi, T., Breyer, M., & Gessler, T. (2021). The (re)politicisation of gender in western europe. European Journal of Politics and Gender, 4, 311–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Acke, H. (2019). Sprachwandel durch feministische Sprachkritik. Zeitschrift Für Literaturwissenschaft und Linguistik, 49, 303–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • AFP (2021). Gendern verbieten? Merz gegen geschlechtsneutrale Sprache. https://www.faz.net/aktuell/politik/inland/gendern-friedrich-merz-gegen-geschlechtsneutrale-sprache-17307811.html (Erstellt: 23. Apr. 2021). Zugegriffen: 6. Mai 2021. FAZ Online.

  • Ängsal, M. P. (2020). Wortkritik in der Feministischen Sprachkritik. In T. Niehr, J. Kilian & J. Schiewe (Hrsg.), Handbuch Sprachkritik (S. 66–72). Suttgart: Metzler.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Aubrey, J. S., & Harrison, K. (2004). The gender-role content of children’s favorite television programs and its links to their gender-related perceptions. Media Psychology, 6, 111–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beller, J., & Kazazi, J. (2013). Is there an effect of gender-fair formulations in the German language? Journal of Unsolved Questions, 3, 5–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Betz, T. (2021). Was bedeutet Gendern? https://www.zdf.de/kinder/logo/gendern-gendergerechte-sprache-geschlechter-100.html (Erstellt: 28. Jan. 2021). Zugegriffen: 25. Juli 2021. ZDF tivi.

  • Blake, C., & Klimmt, C. (2010). Geschlechtergerechte Formulierungen in Nachrichtentexten. Publizistik, 55, 289–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braun, F., Oelkers, S., Rogalski, K., Bosak, J., & Sczesny, S. (2007). „Aus Gründen der Verständlichkeit …“: Der Einfluss generisch maskuliner und alternativer Personenbezeichnungen auf die kognitive Verarbeitung von Texten. Psychologische Rundschau, 58, 183–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braun, F., Sczesny, S., & Stahlberg, D. (2005). Cognitive effects of masculine generics in German: an overview of empirical findings. Communications, 30, 1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burkhart, F. N., & Sigelman, C. K. (1990). Byline bias? Effects of gender on news article evaluations. Journalism Quarterly, 67, 492–500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cameron, J. J., & Stinson, D. A. (2019). Gender (mis)measurement: Guidelines for respecting gender diversity in psychological research. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 13(11), 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diewald, G. (2018). Zur Diskussion: Geschlechtergerechte Sprache als Thema der germanistischen Linguistik – exemplarisch exerziert am Streit um das sogenannte generische Maskulinum. Zeitschrift Für Germanistische Linguistik, 46, 283–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dogruel, L., Joeckel, S., & Wilhelm, C. (2021). Are byline biases an issue of the past? The effect of author’s gender and emotion norm prescriptions on the evaluation of news articles on gender equality. Journalism. https://doi.org/10.1177/14648849211012176. Online First.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Morgan, M., & Signorielli, N. (1982). Charting the mainstream: television’s contributions to political orientations. Journal of Communication, 32, 100–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Götz, M., Hofmann, O., Brosius, H. B., Carter, C., Chan, K., St. Donald, H., Fisherkeller, J., Frenette, M., Kolbjørnsen, T., Lemish, D., Lustyik, K., McMillin, D. C., Walma van der Molen, J. D., Pecora, N., Prinsloo, J., Pestaj, M., Ramos Rivero, P., Mereilles Reis, A.-H., Saeys, F., Scherr, S., & Zhang, H. (2008). Gender in children’s television worldwide. Results from a media analysis in 24 countries. Televizion, 21(E), 4–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Götz, M., Hoffmann, O., Mendel, C., & Velev, M. (2019). Children’s television worldwide II: gender representation in Germany. http://childrens-tv-worldwide.com/pdfs/Germany.pdf. Zugegriffen: 23.08.2021.

  • Günthner, S. (2019). Sprachwissenschaft und Geschlechterforschung: Übermittelt unsere Sprache ein androzentrisches Weltbild? In B. Kortendiek, B. Riegraf & K. Sabisch (Hrsg.), Geschlecht und Gesellschaft. Handbuch interdisziplinäre Geschlechterforschung (Bd. 65, S. 571–579). Wiesbaden: Springer VS.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gygax, P. M., Schoenhals, L., Lévy, A., Luethold, P., & Gabriel, U. (2019). Exploring the onset of a male-biased interpretation of masculine generics among French speaking kindergarten children. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1225. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hanitzsch, T. (2021). Genderstern und Binnen-I: Es ist Zeit, die Realität zu akzeptieren. Publizistik, 66, 181–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Irmen, L., & Linner, U. (2005). Die Repräsentation generisch maskuliner Personenbezeichnungen. Zeitschrift Für Psychologie/Journal of Psychology, 213, 167–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ivanov, C., Lange, M. B., Tiemayer, T., & Ptok, M. (2019). Geschlechtergerechte Sprache in der Wissenschaft: Gebrauch und Motivation. New Perspectives in Gender Research Working Paper Series. https://doi.org/10.3249/2509-8179-gtg-9. Advance online publication.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klimmt, C. (2021). Manipulation betreiben immer nur die anderen. Publizistik, 66, 187–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klimmt, C., Pompetzki, V., & Blake, C. (2008). Geschlechterrepräsentation in Nachrichtentexten. Medien & Kommunikationswissenschaft, 56, 3–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kotthoff, H., & Nübling, D. (2018). Genderlinguistik. Eine Einführung in Sprache, Gespräch und Geschlecht. Tübingen: Narr Francke Attempto.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lünenborg, M., & Maier, T. (2014). Wider die Feminisierung der Öffentlichkeit: Aktuelle Geschlechterordnungen in Journalismus und Politik. Feministische Studien. 32. 234-248. https://doi.org/10.1515/fs-2014-0208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magin, M., & Stark, B. (2010). Mediale Geschlechterstereotype. Publizistik, 55, 383–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pöschko, H., & Prieler, V. (2018). Zur Verständlichkeit und Lesbarkeit von geschlechtergerecht formulierten Schulbuchtexten. Zeitschrift Für Bildungsforschung, 8, 5–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prommer, E., Linke, C., & Stüwe, J. (2017). Is the future equal? Geschlechterrepräsentationen im Kinderfernsehen. Televizion, 30(2), 4–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reimann, M. (2020). Zur Notwendigkeit geschlechtergerechter Sprache im Journalismus. In T. Köhler (Hrsg.), Fake News, Framing, Fact-Checking: Nachrichten im digitalen Zeitalter (S. 283–296). Bielefeld: transcript.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Schmelzer, C. (2013). Gender turn: Einleitung. In C. Schmelzer (Hrsg.), Gender turn : Gesellschaft jenseits der Geschlechternorm (S. 13–20). Bielefeld: transcript.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarzer, M. (2020). Anne Will und der Krieg der Gendersternchen. https://www.rnd.de/medien/geschlechtergerechte-sprache-anne-will-macht-den-gendergap-IC5ACUDVQNCB3KKVYCGLQCEEGA.html (Erstellt: 5. Juni 2020). Zugegriffen: 6. Mai 2021. RND.

  • Springer, N., & Troger, F. (2021). Du stehst unter genauer Beobachtung, unangenehmer Beobachtung. Publizistik, 66, 43–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steiger, V., & Irmen, L. (2011). Recht verständlich und „gender-fair“: Wie sollen Personen in amtlichen Texten bezeichnet werden? Ein Vergleich verschiedener Rezipientengruppen zur Akzeptanz geschlechtergerechter Rechtssprache. Linguistische Berichte, 227, 297–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steiger-Loerbroks, V., & von Stockhausen, L. (2014). Mental representations of gender-fair nouns in German legal language: an eye-movement and questionnaire-based study. Linguistische Berichte, 237, 57–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stöber, R. (2021). Genderstern und Binnen‑I. Zu falscher Symbolpolitik in Zeiten eines zunehmenden Illiberalismus. Publizistik, 66, 11–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thiele, M. (2019). Geschlechterrepräsentationen in den Medien. In M. Karmasin & C. Oggolder (Hrsg.), Österreichische Mediengeschichte (S. 259–276). Wiesbaden: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tiemeyer, T., & Ptok, M. (2018). Gebrauch geschlechtsübergreifender Personenbezeichnungen in der „Sprache · Stimme · Gehör“ im Jahr 2016. Sprache · Stimme · Gehör, 42, 91–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vales, K. (2019). Gegenderte Kindermedien? Na, logo! https://www.genderleicht.de/gendern-bei-kindermedien-kika-und-logo/ (Erstellt: 20. Nov. 2019). Zugegriffen: 6. Mai 2021. Genderleicht.de.

  • Vergoossen, H. P., Renström, E. A., Lindqvist, A., & Gustafsson Sendén, M. (2020). Four dimensions of criticism against gender-fair language. Sex Roles, 83, 328–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vervecken, D., & Hannover, B. (2015). Yes I Can! Social Psychology, 46, 76–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vervecken, D., Gygax, P. M., Gabriel, U., Guillod, M., & Hannover, B. (2015). Warm-hearted businessmen, competitive housewives? Effects of gender-fair language on adolescents’ perceptions of occupations. Frontiers in Psychology. 6. 1437 https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilhelm, C. (2020). Gendered (in)visibility in digital media contexts. Studies in Communication Sciences. https://doi.org/10.24434/j.scoms.2021.01.007. Advance online publication.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sven Jöckel.

Anhang

Anhang

Tab. 5 Anmoderationen Erwachsenen-Studie

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Jöckel, S., Dogruel, L. & Bachofer, R. Wirkung gendersensibler Ansprachen in Anmoderationen bei Erwachsenen und Heranwachsenden. Publizistik 66, 441–462 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11616-021-00682-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11616-021-00682-z

Schlüsselwörter

Keywords

Navigation