INTRODUCTION

It is well documented that patients are not treated equally in our healthcare system: some receive poorer quality of healthcare than others based on their racial/ethnic identity,1,2,3,4 independent of social class. Others, such as older adults5,6 and individuals with low health literacy,7,8 obesity,9,10 and substance use disorders11 may also be viewed negatively by health professionals in a way that adversely impacts their healthcare quality. Implicit bias among clinicians is one factor that perpetuates these disparities.1,12,13 Implicit bias is the automatic activation of stereotypes derived from common cultural experiences, which may override deliberate thought and influence one’s judgment in unintentional and unrecognized ways,2,14,15 and may affect communication behaviors and treatment decisions.3,16,17,18

Clinicians may acquire implicit bias towards patients from one another when communicating verbally or when writing or reading medical records; physicians-in-training may absorb these attitudes as part of the “hidden curriculum” of medical training.19,20,21,22 Few studies have examined the medical record as a mechanism for transmitting bias from one clinician to another. A recent randomized study demonstrated that physicians who read a vignette with the term “substance abuser” as opposed to “having a substance use disorder” were more likely to agree that the depicted character was personally culpable and should have punitive measures taken against him or her.11 Those participants were also less likely to agree with the notion that a “substance abuser” needed treatment as compared to a person “with substance use disorder.”23

In this study, we explored whether stigmatizing language written in a patient medical record was associated with a subsequent physician-in-training’s attitudes towards the patient and clinical decision-making. We hypothesized that resident physicians and medical students who read a chart note using stigmatizing language to describe a patient would have more negative attitudes about the patient and would treat the patient’s pain less aggressively than those who read a chart note using neutral language.

METHODS

We employed an experimental vignette study design in which residents and medical students were randomized to read one of two chart notes presenting medically identical information about a hypothetical patient with sickle cell disease (SCD). The first chart note utilized stigmatizing language and details; the second used neutral language. After reading the chart note, participants completed a survey assessing their attitudes regarding the patient (residents and students) and treatment decisions (residents only) for the patient. We focused on a patient with SCD, as there is significant evidence that these patients are stigmatized by clinicians and experience inadequate pain management, conflicts with staff, and lack of respect.24 We decided to focus on medical students and residents, as training is a time of socialization through the “hidden curriculum”19 and thus a potential point of intervention. We selected residents in emergency medicine and internal medicine because they care for patients with SCD presenting with pain crises. Hypothetical chart notes were used because vignettes are a robust and efficient method to systematically assess variation in opinion and decision-making, without confounding by other patient characteristics.25,26 All research activities were approved by the academic medical center’s institutional review board.

Study Setting and Sample

The study was performed at a large, urban academic medical center. Residents in the medical center’s two internal medicine (IM) programs and one emergency medicine (EM) program were recruited via emails cosigned by their program directors. Medical students were recruited with an email from a student on the study team, cosigned by the principal investigator. The survey was administered online via Qualtrics, which randomized participants to the stigmatizing language or neutral language arm of the study. As incentive for their participation, residents were given $25 gift cards. Medical students were entered into a lottery for one of five $100 gift cards. Two reminders were sent via email at 10-day intervals.

Survey Vignettes

Two vignettes were created by the study team, written in the format of chart notes. The vignette describes Mr. R, a hypothetical 28-year-old man with SCD who presents to the emergency department (ED) with a vaso-occlusive crisis. In Section 1 of the vignette, the admitting physician describes Mr. R’s chief complaint, history of present illness, and physical exam findings. In Section 2, a nurse documents information about Mr. R from an hour later. The chart notes are shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Text Employed in the Vignettes

The two chart notes differed in the use of stigmatizing versus neutral language. The stigmatizing chart note was a written composite based on text abstracted from medical records from the same medical center.27 We defined stigma after Bruce Link and Jo Phelan, who conceptualize stigma as elements of labeling, stereotyping, separating, status loss, and discrimination co-occurring in a power situation that allows these processes to unfold.28 Stigmatizing language in patient charts was characterized by three linguistic features, as elaborated in prior work27: casting doubt on the patient’s pain (e.g. insisting that his pain is “still a 10” vs. still has 10/10 pain), portraying the patient negatively (with irrelevant or unnecessary indicators of lower socioeconomic status such as hanging out with friends outside McDonald’s), and implying patient responsibility with references to uncooperativeness (e.g. he refuses his oxygen mask vs. he is not tolerating the oxygen mask). In addition, we included linguistic variations such as using the term “narcotic” in the stigmatizing chart note vs. “opioid” in the neutral note. The neutral chart note was written by the study team to serve as a comparison against the stigmatizing language note, while remaining realistic.

The survey was reviewed, piloted by members of the study team and ten medical students, and revised based on feedback. Pilot data responses were not included in the final data set.

Primary Outcomes

Pain Management Intensity

After reading Section 1, residents were asked to choose among four options for pain medication dosing (ketorolac 60 mg IM; morphine 4 mg IV; hydromorphone 1 mg IV; hydromorphone 4 mg IV). They were then presented with Section 2 and asked how they would adjust their management plan (change to oral pain medication and discharge; give second dose of IV medication; increase dose of IV medication; tell patient it is too early for additional opioids and offer ketorolac). The two medication management questions were combined (and the options for morphine 4 mg IV and hydromorphone 1 mg IV collapsed based on clinical equivalence) to yield a score from 2 to 7, with higher scores indicating more aggressive pain management. Medical students read both sections of the chart note at the same time and did not answer the pain management questions.

Attitudes towards the Patient

All study participants completed an abbreviated Positive Attitudes towards Sickle Cell Patients Scale (PASS).24 Seven of the 10 items of the PASS were utilized to directly assess attitudes towards the hypothetical patient, using a five-point Likert scale for a total score range of 7–35, with higher scores representing more positive attitudes. Because the PASS was developed as a scale to measure attitudes towards actual patients rather than hypothetical patients, we did not view the remaining three items (which ask the respondent to indicate how much they respect, empathize, and like the patient) as adaptable to the vignette scenario.

Secondary Outcome

Perceptions of Vignette Physician Attitudes

To assess whether participants perceived bias within the note, the other three PASS items were modified to ask the participant to rate how much the admitting physician liked, respected, and empathized with the patient described (vs. participants rating their own attitudes towards the patient). These three questions were combined into a score ranging from 3 to 15, with higher scores indicating that the respondent perceived more positive attitudes by the admitting physician towards the patient. After these questions, and without the possibility of returning to change their answer, an additional, open-ended question asked the respondent to indicate what features of the note might convey positive or negative attitudes towards the patient.

Other Predictor Variables

Comfort in Dosing Pain Medication

The residents rated their comfort in dosing pain medication on a scale from 1 to 10, with a higher score representing greater comfort.

Demographics

We collected information from respondents about their year of training (in medical school or residency), residency type (IM vs. EM), gender, race, and ethnicity.

Statistical Analysis

We used chi-square analyses to compare characteristics of respondents between the two study arms. After noting no significant differences, we used t tests (for pain management intensity, normally distributed) and Wilcoxon rank-sum tests (for attitudes towards patients, not normally distributed) to calculate differences between arms.

We conducted several secondary analyses. We performed t tests to assess potential differences between students and residents, between black and non-black respondents, and between EM and IM residents in attitudes towards the patient. To evaluate whether residents who were more comfortable dosing pain medication chose more aggressive pain management, we performed a linear regression between the medication score and comfort score among all residents and among residents within each study arm. Finally, we explored respondents’ perceptions of bias on the part of the (hypothetical) admitting physician using a Wilcoxon rank-sum test to calculate the difference in the three-item attitude score—how much respondents perceived that the admitting physician liked and had respect and empathy for the patient—between the stigmatizing and neutral language chart notes. We explored and described the reasons given in the open-ended comments.

RESULTS

Study Sample

Of the 413 study participants (54% response rate), 42.8% were female, 43.5% were residents, and 14% were Hispanic/Latino. Most respondents were white (54.7%), 26.9% were Asian, and 10.4% identified as black or African American. Respondent characteristics including year of training, ethnicity, gender, and race were balanced between study arms (Table 2).

Table 2 Characteristics of Survey Respondents

Impact of the Stigmatizing Language Chart Note on Attitudes and Pain Management

Differences in attitudes and pain management intensity between arms are shown in Figure 1a and b. Exposure to the stigmatizing language chart note negatively influenced resident and medical student attitudes towards the hypothetical patient as measured by the PASS scores: all respondents (20.3 stigmatizing language vs. 25.1 neutral language, p < 0.001; Fig. 1a). This was true of medical students (21.6 stigmatizing vs. 26.6 neutral, p < 0.001) and residents (18.7 stigmatizing vs. 23.0 neutral, p < 0.001).

Figure 1
figure 1

Effect of stigmatizing language on attitudes (Panel A) and on pain management (Panel B)

Resident physicians who read the stigmatizing language chart note prescribed pain medication less aggressively than those who read the neutral language chart note (4.7 vs. 5.3, p < 0.001; Fig. 1b).

Additional Predictors of Respondent Attitudes

Residents’ attitudes towards the hypothetical patient were more negative than those of medical students, regardless of study arm (PASS score 20.8 residents vs. 24.2 medical students, p < 0.001), and there was an inverse correlation between year of training and PASS score (coefficient −0.95, p < 0.001). Attitudes towards the patient were more positive among respondents who identified as black than among those who did not (24.4 vs. 22.5, p = 0.04). There was no difference in PASS scores between EM and IM residents.

Comfort in Dosing Pain Medication

In respondents exposed to the neutral language chart note, comfort in dosing pain medication correlated with more aggressive pain management (coefficient 0.09, p = 0.04); however, this correlation was not present in respondents who read the stigmatizing language chart note (coefficient 0.03, p = 0.50).

Perceptions of Vignette Physician Attitudes

Respondents who read the stigmatizing language chart note perceived that the physician who wrote the note had more negative attitudes towards the patient, relative to those who read the neutral language chart note (7.6 stigmatizing language vs. 9.2 neutral language, p < 0.001). The vast majority (88%) of participants who read the neutral language chart note thought the physician had neutral or positive attitudes towards the patient (score ≥9). Of those who read the stigmatizing language chart note, only 45% felt the physician had neutral or positive attitudes towards the patient.

Of the approximately 20 differences between the chart notes, almost all were highlighted by respondents who read the stigmatizing language note and perceived that the physician had negative attitudes towards the patient (score < 9). Frequencies of each linguistic feature that were mentioned by at least five respondents are listed in Table 3. Frequently mentioned phrases among those who read the stigmatizing chart note and perceived a negative attitude were the words “narcotic dependent” and “in our ED frequently,” as well as the mention of the girlfriend having shoes on the bed and requesting a bus token. Those who read the neutral note and perceived it to be neutral highlighted the fact that the patient was described as being “in distress” and noted that the girlfriend was by his side.

Table 3 What Did Participants Notice about Each Chart Note? Examples from the Vignettes and Frequency of Mentions by Participants

DISCUSSION

Language may play a powerful role in influencing clinician attitudes and behavior. Our results suggest that language used in medical records to describe patients can directly influence subsequent physicians-in-training who read the notes, in terms of both their attitudes towards the patient and their medication-prescribing behavior. This is an important and overlooked pathway by which bias can be propagated from one clinician to another, further impairing healthcare quality for the individual patient as well as entrenching healthcare disparities overall for those who are stigmatized.

There is growing evidence that the language used to communicate in healthcare reflects and influences clinician attitudes towards their patients.11,23,29,30 Finucane and Carrese found that residents were more likely to mention black race (vs. white) when the patient had stereotypically negative behavior (e.g. drug use or treatment nonadherence).29 Specific to SCD, Glassberg et al. found that physicians who use the term “sickler”—a term to describe patients with SCD, which is often considered by patients and clinicians to be derogatory—have more negative attitudes towards those patients.30 Our study adds to this compelling literature by highlighting the important role of language in the medical record as more than a reflection of attitudes—it is also a means to transmit these attitudes to others.

We found that residents had more negative attitudes than medical students towards the patient. It is notable that attitudes towards patients seem to become more negative as trainees progress; this may be a manifestation of the ethical erosion trainees can experience31 as they are influenced by the “hidden curriculum” of observed negative attitudes and behaviors among their peers and seniors in the clinical setting.19,20,21,22 We also observed that participants who identified as black or African American had more positive attitudes towards the patient, affirming prior findings that African American clinicians have more positive attitudes towards patients with SCD.13

While respondents noted a variety of details that led them to believe that the admitting physician was more or less biased against the patient, a few themes stood out. Some respondents commented on the negative impact of irrelevant comments about behavior and conflicts with the healthcare team, while others noted the positive impact of including psychosocial factors and patient beliefs. A noteworthy finding was the tension in quoting a patient’s own words: some respondents felt it was an empathetic practice, while others felt quotes were employed to signal low socioeconomic status or imply factitious histories. The practice of quoting a patient, often encouraged in medical education,32,33 deserves a more nuanced analysis of its potential role in providing information and nurturing compassion versus stigmatizing a patient and perpetuating bias. It is remarkable that, when asked to reflect on the note, many respondents were able to identify potentially impactful language—even if earlier they had made their own judgments based on that language. This capacity for reflection may be a promising point of intervention.

One important issue to consider moving forward is what should be documented in the medical record. We have shown that language in medical records may perpetuate bias by negatively impacting the attitudes and decision-making of subsequent physicians-in-training. Yet some of the information conveyed in stigmatizing notes—e.g. concerns about a substance use disorder or nonadherence—is important to share with members of the healthcare team. Our study did not aim to address the issue of exactly what should be written, but to understand and consider the consequences. Physicians-in-training may feel both positive and negative emotions about their patients, but value judgments informed by feelings about a patient should not appear in the medical record. Clinicians have a responsibility to record data that are relevant and as objective as possible, as well as to recognize that using certain phrases (e.g. substance abuse vs. substance use) opens the possibility for other clinicians to make or interpret judgment.

This study has several limitations. First, data collection occurred at a single center: a large, urban academic medical center with a significant black patient population. Results may not be generalizable to other healthcare settings. Second, this was a vignette study with a defined set of answer choices to assess attitudes and pain management, so we do not know exactly how these dynamics may manifest in everyday practice. However, the attitudinal scale used was previously validated, and the choices for pain management were made by physicians with expertise in SCD and emergency and inpatient medicine. Importantly, all of the stigmatizing language used to construct the vignette was abstracted from real medical records, so the vignette chart note represents a realistic portrayal of how clinicians may communicate with each other in the medical record. Our chart note had many examples of stigmatizing language; thus, the effect may be stronger than typically seen in a single note.

Finally, the current study did not set out to address which dimensions of the stigmatizing language were the most impactful (e.g. casting doubt using quotation marks vs. blaming the patient for his condition). This was a proof-of-concept study to demonstrate that the language we use in medical records impacts physician-in-training attitudes and decision-making. Future work should evaluate which kinds of stigmatizing language are most likely to perpetuate bias and adversely affect patient care.

Every encounter with a patient is documented in a chart note, including patient symptoms and history, objective signs and results, and the clinician’s assessment and plan for the patient. For some patients, the medical record may be the only source of information a new clinician has about them. We must question the assumption that the medical record always represents an objective space. Clinicians must be vigilant to guard against contributing to bias as they write chart notes about their own patients and as they read chart notes written by others. The language in medical records should be more carefully considered to avoid perpetuating clinician biases and the healthcare disparities that may arise from them.