Abstract
Background
Overuse of healthcare is pervasive in the United States, often exposing patients to harm with little likelihood of benefit. Older Americans are particularly vulnerable to overuse and impacted by it, yet it is unknown whether older patients perceive overuse as a consequential problem.
Objective
To explore the experiences and perspectives of older adults with respect to healthcare overuse in order to develop a framework for understanding and reducing overuse in older adults.
Design
Qualitative study using focus group methodology.
Participants
Five focus groups were held with people ≥65 years of age (N = 38) in four senior centers in Baltimore, Maryland, in 2016.
Approach
Transcripts were analyzed using qualitative content analysis to identify major themes.
Key Results
Of the 38 participants, 28 were women and 29 were African-American; 31 had at least a 12th grade education. While virtually all reported experience with what they perceived to have been healthcare overuse, some expressed concern that they had been denied appropriate care. They perceived overuse to have occurred when interventions were applied in the absence of symptoms (excluding cancer screening), did not improve symptoms, were discordant with their preferences, or were duplicative. Some defined overuse as interventions that were offered before less intensive options or too early in the course of disease. Suggested contributors to overuse were poor quality communication between patients and healthcare providers, and between different healthcare providers. Participants reported suffering from treatment effects, high costs, worry, and inconvenience from what they perceived to be overuse. They suggested that overuse may be reduced when the patient is involved in decision making and has a trusted primary care doctor.
Conclusions
The experience of older adults highlights potential sites of intervention to reduce healthcare overuse. Engaging patients in shared decision making and enhancing communication and knowledge transfer should be tested as interventions to reduce perceived overuse.
Similar content being viewed by others
Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.
The continued growth in healthcare spending in the United States is unsustainable.1 Moreover, spending is not correlated with indicators of high-quality care.2 , 3 This disparity between costs and health outcomes has led many to conclude that healthcare services are overused in the U.S.4,5, – 6
Healthcare overuse has been defined as the provision of care in circumstances where the potential for harm exceeds the potential for benefit.7 Excess use of health services puts patients at risk of adverse outcomes6 , 8,9, – 10 and creates system-wide detriments.11 Overuse is pervasive within the healthcare system, and is a target of recent efforts such as the Choosing Wisely campaign of the American Board of Internal Medicine (ABIM) Foundation. We suggest that older Americans may be particularly vulnerable to this phenomenon. Examples of overuse include cancer screening tests in patients with limited life expectancy,12 treatments delivered at end of life that do not impact disease course or quality of life,13 therapies delivered without recognition of the heterogeneity among older patients,14 polypharmacy in multimorbid older patients,15 and care coordination failures when multiple specialists are involved.16 Given that the harms and benefits of interventions are often unclear, and patients are frequently unaware of these trade-offs, their perceptions of what constitutes overuse may differ from what clinicians or professional societies define as overuse.
We were interested in learning whether older adults perceive that there is overuse of healthcare services and whether they view it as problematic. Our goal was to illuminate the perspectives of older adults as a necessary first step to informing a framework for understanding, and ultimately reducing, healthcare overuse in older adults.
METHODS
Study Design
We used focus group methodology. The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine Institutional Review Board approved the study. Participants provided written informed consent.
Recruitment
We contacted coordinators at senior activity centers across Baltimore City to assess their willingness to have their centers be participating sites. We asked the coordinators to display a flyer inviting participants to attend a focus group at a specified date and time. The coordinators were made aware of our inclusion and exclusion criteria: participants needed to be over age 65, without significant dementia, and English-speaking. Each group was limited to fewer than ten participants.
Discussion Guide
We began by interviewing three older adults to inform development of our discussion guide (Appendix 1). The interviews probed their understanding of the concept of overuse, explored wording choices, and gathered preliminary content to explore in greater depth. The discussion guide was iteratively refined to include open-ended questions about use and overuse of medical services. To prompt discussion, it also included descriptions of two scenarios of overuse: (1) “the doctor recommends a colonoscopy to screen for colon cancer in a sickly, 90-year-old man”; and (2) “the doctor prescribes diabetes medicines to a sickly, 90-year-old man to get his blood sugar into the normal range.”
Focus Group Sessions
We used purposive sampling to achieve diversity with regard to race/ethnicity and socioeconomic status. All sessions were held in private rooms at the centers, and each session lasted approximately 75 min. Paired investigators moderated the audiotaped sessions (ARG and JBS for four sessions; ARG and MT for the fifth). One of the investigators, ARG, had prior experience moderating one-on-one interviews and focus groups. We used a brief questionnaire to collect demographic information from participants. Participants received a $25 gift card as compensation.
At the beginning of each session, we stated that our purpose was “to learn more about how people make decisions about their healthcare,” and that “we particularly wanted to learn about something that has been in the news a lot recently – the concept of overtreatment, or harmful medical care. The terms “unnecessary care,” “excessive care,” and “overtreatment” were used interchangeably, after we instructed the participants to consider them equivalent.
The discussion explored participants’ perceptions of whether they or an acquaintance had experienced overuse, the consequences of overuse, or factors contributing to overuse, and how participants make decisions about their healthcare. After each session, the investigators reviewed the major themes emerging from the discussion, and the questions were iteratively refined as needed.
Analysis
The audiotaped transcripts were professionally transcribed verbatim (Ubiqus; www.ubiqus.com). We used a combination of conventional and directed approaches for qualitative content analysis.17 The conventional approach is appropriate when there is little existing theory about a phenomenon, as was largely true in this case. Rather than using preconceived categories, we allowed coding categories to flow from the data. With a directed approach, coding starts with an existing theory, and themes that either support or refute this theory emerge from the data. We expected that social influence would be a prominent theme and therefore developed a small set of codes to capture the social relationships contributing to overuse. Focus groups were added until the team members felt that thematic saturation had been reached.18
To begin, all three investigators independently reviewed one transcript and inductively developed codes to describe important topics. The investigators then agreed upon a preliminary coding scheme. Each investigator coded transcripts independently, and then the three met as a group in weekly sessions to achieve 100% consensus about the final coding of each transcript. The coding was revised iteratively as needed. We used ATLAS.ti version 8 textual data analysis software (ATLAS.ti Scientific Software Development, Berlin, Germany).
RESULTS
We held five focus groups in December 2016 at four senior centers in Baltimore City. The number of participants per group ranged from six to nine, with a total of 38 participants. Three of the centers were in lower-income, predominately black neighborhoods, and one was in a mixed-income, predominately white neighborhood. Of the 38 participants, 28 were women and 29 were African-American; 31 had at least a 12th grade education. Seven rated their health as fair, 20 as good, and 11 as very good or excellent. Virtually all of the participants reported that they or an acquaintance had experienced healthcare overuse. Content analysis revealed five major themes, presented below with representative quotes.
Theme 1: Definitions of Overuse
Participants described scenarios that they considered to be examples of healthcare overuse (Table 1). Screening tests were considered overuse by some participants, who did not feel that doctors should look for asymptomatic problems. Other participants were receptive to the idea of screening, at least for cancer, which appeared more threatening to them than conditions such as diabetes. This is illustrated by the following example:
My doctor is telling me that it’s important because… my brother had it… But she leaves it up to me. She [doesn’t] make that decision for me… she just said, “Let me know when you’re ready.” I haven’t been ready in the last 6 years… I think it’s important… But I just don’t like the thought of it.
Participants defined overuse as intensive or invasive interventions that were offered as the first recommended treatment or too early in the disease course. For example:
I went to see an orthopedic doctor for the first time because I have arthritis in my knee…Her first response was, "Has anyone spoken to you about a knee replacement?" She didn't even stop to talk about other options.
Participants also defined overuse as treatments that do not cure illness and must be taken chronically. Many participants cited duplicative testing—in particular, frequent blood testing—as an example of overuse. Participants were likely to perceive laboratory testing as overuse when it was offered without explanation.
Theme 2: Consequences of Overuse
Participants reported that they or acquaintances had suffered physical harms from interventions that they, in retrospect, perceived to be overuse (Table 2). Tests and treatments that led to no perceptible benefits for the patient were described as overuse. For example, one participant reacted to the hypothetical scenario of a frail 90-year-old man being prescribed antidiabetic agents to achieve normal blood glucose levels:
They’ll keep prescribing things over and over and over, and it’s not doing anything for us. It’s just making us feel bad.
Another consequence of perceived overuse was patient discomfort from high medication burden or reliance on electronic devices. In addition to physical and psychic pain, participants cited high out-of-pocket costs as a consequence of perceived overuse. One participant observed that even when patients are shielded from high out-of-pocket costs, they pay the price of overuse because insurance companies pass the costs on to patients through increased premiums:
I’m paying—no matter what it is, I’m paying the cost. When the cost goes up, my premiums and everything go up.
Theme 3: Contributors to Overuse
Participants identified several contributors to overuse (Table 3). The most commonly cited contributor was inadequate communication between healthcare providers and patients, and between healthcare providers involved in a patient’s care. Many participants described poor patient–doctor communication that led them to have unrealistic expectations of benefits and to choose treatments that they might not have chosen if they had been well informed. Participants also expressed the belief that overuse happens because of inadequate communication between different healthcare providers involved in a patient’s care.
Many participants said that financial incentives for physicians and for insurance and pharmaceutical companies are a driver of overuse. Lastly, participants expressed the belief that conceptualizing a procedure as “routine” may lead to overuse.
Theme 4: Strategies for Reducing Overuse
Several potential strategies emerged for reducing overuse (Table 4). Some participants suggested that overuse is less likely when they are involved in shared decision making with the clinician. Although some participants expressed the belief that primary care visits are too short to be meaningful (“I don’t see the purpose of primary doctor now, because you only got 15 minutes. What can you do in 15 minutes?”), many indicated that having a trusted clinician—usually a primary care provider—reduced the likelihood that overuse would occur.
Theme 5: Underuse of Healthcare
Though the topic of the focus groups was “excessive or unnecessary medical care,” many participants expressed concern that they had been denied care that they deemed appropriate, such as imaging studies or referrals to specialists. For example, one participant observed, “They [the primary care doctor] need to send you to a specialist and not try to do it themselves. Get those tests so they can find out what’s wrong.” Some participants also expressed dissatisfaction with efforts to reduce unnecessary emergency department visits, and a belief that doctors profit by keeping patients out of emergency departments.
DISCUSSION
We have characterized the experiences and perspectives of older adults with respect to their perceived overuse of healthcare services, to begin developing a framework for understanding and reducing overuse in older adults. We found that the majority of participants perceived that they had experienced overuse and found it problematic. They offered several definitions of overuse and reported suffering physical harms, anxiety, financial cost and inconvenience from what they perceived to be overuse. When we asked participants to reflect on the causes of overuse, the most frequently cited contributor was poor quality communication—between patients and healthcare providers, and between healthcare providers involved in a patient’s care. They suggested that overuse may be reduced when the patient is involved in decision making and has a trusted primary care doctor.
Participants perceived overuse when interventions were offered without adequate explanation, or when screening tests for chronic conditions such as type 2 diabetes were performed in the absence of symptoms. These findings highlight the need to improve communication between patients and healthcare providers.19 There is no established best practice for how to discuss the rationale for laboratory tests or imaging studies with patients; time pressures on clinicians are a barrier to these discussions. The Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality has online toolkits for healthcare providers, which include templates designed to improve communication and increase patient understanding of health information, but it is not known how widely these are used or their effects.20 Such tools could be evaluated in future studies to develop best practices on how to discuss the rationale behind laboratory tests and imaging studies with patients, or how best to communicate with patients in after-visit summaries.
Participants perceived overuse, in retrospect, when interventions did not improve their symptoms or when extended treatment regimens were required. They also perceived overuse when aggressive care was delivered at the end of life that did not improve quality of life or alter disease course. This may reflect another consequence of poor communication—patients perceive overuse because they are not being prepared to understand that uncertainty is part of medicine. As Simpkin and Schwartzstein note, doctors “make decisions on the basis of imperfect data and limited knowledge, which leads to diagnostic uncertainty, coupled with the uncertainty that arises from unpredictable patient responses to treatment and…outcomes that are far from binary.”21 We wonder whether this needs to be better conveyed to patients to enable them to adjust their expectations from clinical encounters.22
Although we reminded participants that the topic was overuse of healthcare, the conversation in each group often returned to participants’ frustration with the healthcare system. Some participants revealed mistrust of healthcare providers and a perception that the doctor–patient relationship is unequal23; these beliefs appeared to be fueled by current events, such as the EpiPen price spike24 and newspaper reports about a cardiologist who implanted hundreds of unnecessary cardiac stents.25 Some participants viewed efforts to reduce overuse—for example, by preventing unnecessary use of emergency departments—with skepticism. Indeed, while virtually all participants related that they or an acquaintance had experienced over-testing or overtreatment, many participants also expressed concern that they had been denied appropriate care. This finding may be important for predicting the impact of efforts to reduce overuse, such as the Choosing Wisely initiative.26,27, – 28
Our study has some limitations. Focus groups rely on self-report and are prone to recall and social desirability biases. As with most qualitative research, the study was not designed to be representative of all older adults. Four of the five group sessions were held in lower-income, predominately black neighborhoods. Participants may have had below-average health literacy. In addition, they were community-dwelling older adults and well enough to participate in senior center activities; people in worse overall health or with acute symptoms may have different views on healthcare overuse. Although some degree of researcher bias is inevitable, we took several steps to minimize the influence of selective perception and interpretive bias: We wrote the interview guide with patient involvement, used independent coding of transcripts and triangulation among three researchers, and sought to identify and report discrepant evidence (i.e., quotes about underuse). Lastly, we were not able to confirm participants’ experiences. Thus, we cannot say whether overuse occurred, only that participants perceived overuse to have occurred. Patients’ perceptions of overuse may have important consequences: if people perceive that testing performed in the absence of symptoms constitutes overuse, they may defer screening. Conversely, if there is pervasive mistrust of healthcare providers, initiatives to prevent overuse are likely to be viewed as efforts to deny appropriate care.
CONCLUSIONS
Older adults identified numerous possible contributors to overuse. We encourage larger, nationally representative studies to test whether what was communicated by these participants are in fact determinants of overuse. If the findings of the present study are confirmed, future work should address the contributors to overuse, possibly by improving communication training for healthcare providers and increasing resources and incentives for clinicians to engage patients in shared decision making. In addition, policy reforms to minimize financial drivers of overuse will undoubtedly be needed—for example, the removal of non-evidence-based interventions from coverage schedules and the realignment of incentives away from volume and toward quality of outcomes.29
References
Keehan SP, Stone DA, Poisal JA, Cuckler GA, Sisko AM, Smith SD, et al. National Health Expenditure Projections, 2016-25: Price Increases, Aging Push Sector To 20 Percent Of Economy. Health Aff (Millwood). 2017;36(3):553–63.
Fisher ES, Wennberg JE. Health care quality, geographic variations, and the challenge of supply-sensitive care. Perspect Biol Med. 2003;46(1):69–79.
Starfield B. Is US health really the best in the world? JAMA. 2000;284(4):483–5.
Reinhardt UE, Hussey PS, Anderson GF. U.S. health care spending in an international context. Health Aff (Millwood). 2004;23(3):10–25.
Korenstein D, Falk R, Howell EA, Bishop T, Keyhani S. Overuse of health care services in the United States: an understudied problem. Arch Intern Med. 2012;172(2):171–8.
Grady D, Redberg RF. Less is more: how less health care can result in better health. Arch Intern Med. 2010;170(9):749–50.
Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality. Glossary: Underuse, overuse, misuse. Available at: https://psnet.ahrq.gov/glossary/u. Acccessed 26 June 2017.
Warren JL, Klabunde CN, Mariotto AB, Meekins A, Topor M, Brown ML, et al. Adverse events after outpatient colonoscopy in the Medicare population. Ann Intern Med. 2009;150(12):849–57, W152.
Zhang Y, Lee BY, Donohue JM. Ambulatory antibiotic use and prescription drug coverage in older adults. Arch Intern Med. 2010;170(15):1308–14.
Amin AP, Spertus JA, Cohen DJ, Chhatriwalla A, Kennedy KF, Vilain K, et al. Use of drug-eluting stents as a function of predicted benefit: clinical and economic implications of current practice. Arch Intern Med. 2012;172(15):1145–52.
Berwick DM, Hackbarth AD. Eliminating waste in US health care. JAMA. 2012;307(14):1513–6.
Redberg R, Katz M, Grady D. Diagnostic tests: another frontier for less is more: or why talking to your patient is a safe and effective method of reassurance. Arch Intern Med. 2011;171(7):619.
Mack JW, Cronin A, Keating NL, Taback N, Huskamp HA, Malin JL, et al. Associations between end-of-life discussion characteristics and care received near death: a prospective cohort study. J Clin Oncol. 2012;30(35):4387–95.
Braithwaite RS, Fiellin D, Justice AC. The payoff time: a flexible framework to help clinicians decide when patients with comorbid disease are not likely to benefit from practice guidelines. Med Care. 2009;47(6):610–7.
Al Hamid A, Ghaleb M, Aljadhey H, Aslanpour Z. A systematic review of hospitalization resulting from medicine-related problems in adult patients. Br J Clin Pharmacol. 2014;78(2):202–17.
Liu CW, Einstadter D, Cebul RD. Care fragmentation and emergency department use among complex patients with diabetes. Am J Manag Care. 2010;16(6):413–20.
Hsieh HF, Shannon SE. Three approaches to qualitative content analysis. Qual Health Res. 2005;15(9):1277–88.
Crabtree BF, Miller W.L. Doing Qualitative Research. 2nd ed. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications; 1999.
Krouss M, Croft L, Morgan DJ. Physician Understanding and Ability to Communicate Harms and Benefits of Common Medical Treatments. JAMA Intern Med. 2016;176(10):1565–67.
Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality. AHRQ Health Literacy Universal Precautions Toolkit. Available at: https://www.ahrq.gov/professionals/quality-patient-safety/quality-resources/tools/literacy-toolkit/index.html. Accessed 26 June 2017.
Simpkin AL, Schwartzstein RM. Tolerating Uncertainty - The Next Medical Revolution? NEJM. 2016;375(18):1713–5.
Matlock DD, Mandrola JM. The antidote for unprepared patients: a caring clinician. JAMA Intern Med. 2014;174(1):86–7.
Nimmon L, Stenfors-Hayes T. The "Handling" of power in the physician-patient encounter: perceptions from experienced physicians. BMC Med Educ. 2016;16:114.
Thomas K. Mylan Chief’s Answers on EpiPen Frustrate House Panel. The New York Times. September 22, 2016: B3.
Bishop T. Judge upholds removal of Mark Midei’s medical license. The Baltimore Sun. May 8, 2012:2A.
Stepanczuk C, Williams N, Morrison K, Kemmerer C. Factors influencing patients' receptiveness to evidence-based recommendations during the clinical encounter. J Comp Eff Res. 2017. https://doi.org/10.2217/cer-2016-0077.
Keyhani S, Cheng EM, Naseri A, Halm EA, Williams LS, Johanning J, et al. Common Reasons That Asymptomatic Patients Who Are 65 Years and Older Receive Carotid Imaging. JAMA Intern Med. 2016;176(5):626–33.
Parks AL, O'Malley PG. From Choosing Wisely to Practicing Value-More to the Story. JAMA Intern Med. 2016;176(10):1571–2.
Elshaug AG, Rosenthal MB, Lavis JN, Brownlee S, Schmidt H, Nagpal S, et al. Levers for addressing medical underuse and overuse: achieving high-value health care. Lancet. 2017. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(16)32586-7.
Prior Presentations
This study was presented at the 5th Lown Institute Conference in Boston, MA, and at the 2017 Society of General Internal Medicine Annual Meeting in Washington, DC.
Funding
Supported by K24 AG049036-01A1 from the National Institute on Aging (Segal).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that they do not have a conflict of interest.
Electronic supplementary material
ESM 1
(DOCX 20 kb)
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Green, A.R., Tung, M. & Segal, J.B. Older Adults’ Perceptions of the Causes and Consequences of Healthcare Overuse: A Qualitative Study. J GEN INTERN MED 33, 892–897 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11606-017-4264-y
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11606-017-4264-y