Abstract
Background
Although previous studies suggest an association between provider burnout and suboptimal self-reported communication, no studies relate physician burnout to observed patient-physician communication behaviors.
Objective
To investigate the relationship between physician burnout and observed patient-physician communication outcomes in patient-physician encounters.
Design
Longitudinal study of enrollment data from a trial of interventions to improve patient adherence to hypertension treatment.
Setting
Fifteen urban community-based clinics in Baltimore, MD.
Participants
Forty physicians and 235 of their adult hypertensive patients, with oversampling of ethnic minorities and poor persons. Fifty-three percent of physicians were women, and the average practice experience was 11.2 years. Among the 235 patients, 66% were women, 60% were African-American, and 90% were insured.
Measurements
Audiotape analysis of communication during outpatient encounters (one per patient) using the Roter Interaction Analysis System and patients’ ratings of satisfaction with and trust and confidence in the physician.
Results
The median time between the physician burnout assessment and the patient encounter was 15.1 months (range 5.6–30). Multivariate analyses revealed no significant differences in physician communication based on physician burnout. However, compared with patients of low-burnout physicians, patients of high-burnout physicians gave twice as many negative rapport-building statements (incident risk ratio 2.06, 95% CI 1.58 – 2.86, p < 0.001). Physician burnout was not significantly associated with physician or patient affect, patient-centeredness, verbal dominance, or length of the encounter. Physician burnout was also not significantly associated with patients’ ratings of their satisfaction, confidence, or trust.
Conclusions
Physician burnout was not associated with physician communication behaviors nor with most measures of patient-centered communication. However, patients engaged in more rapport-building behaviors. These findings suggest a complex relationship between physician burnout and patient-physician communication, which should be investigated and linked to patient outcomes in future research.
Similar content being viewed by others
Explore related subjects
Discover the latest articles, news and stories from top researchers in related subjects.Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.
INTRODUCTION
Evidence from the primary care setting suggests that the quality of patient-provider relationships and communication are related to positive patient outcomes. Studies directly measuring observed patient-provider encounters suggest that higher quality communication influences patient satisfaction, compliance, and recall of information.1–3 Furthermore, patient-provider communication is linked to improved chronic disease health outcomes, such as emotional health, functioning, symptom resolution, and blood pressure and glucose control.4–6
Several studies have suggested that provider factors may affect the quality of patient-provider communication, including age, gender, patient-provider race concordance, and provider attitudes toward patients.2,7–13 However, fewer studies have investigated how physician well-being may relate to communication behaviors in patient encounters. Physician burnout – a syndrome of depersonalization, emotional exhaustion, and reduced sense of accomplishment14 – may affect the quality of patient-provider communication. A large survey of primary care physicians and medical subspecialists found that 22% of physicians met criteria for burnout.15
Most studies about physician burnout have assessed its relationship with physician self-reported attitudes (such as confidence in communication, empathy, or perceived reciprocity in the patient-physician relationship) and behaviors (such as propensity for medical errors or quality of care delivered).15–20 Few studies link burnout among health professionals to patient outcomes, such as satisfaction or functional recovery after hospitalization,21,22 and we found no studies linking physician burnout to particular healthcare processes. To our knowledge, no study has directly examined the relationship between physician burnout and observable communication behaviors in patient-physician encounters.
The objective of this study was to investigate the relationship between primary care physician burnout and their subsequent communication with hypertensive patients in routine medical visits. Specifically, we hypothesized that professional burnout would diminish physicians’ inclination or ability to engage in rapport-building with their patients during routine medical visits. As secondary outcomes, we hypothesized that higher levels of burnout would be associated with less positive affect by physicians and patients, less patient-centeredness, shorter visit lengths, greater verbal dominance by physicians, and more negative patient ratings of satisfaction, trust, and confidence in their physicians.
METHODS
Study Design and Sample
We conducted a longitudinal study using enrollment data from the Patient-Physician Partnership (PPP) Study, a randomized controlled trial of patient and physician interventions to improve adherence to therapy among hypertensive minority patients.23 For this analysis, we combined the control and intervention groups into one cohort.
Our sample population was drawn from 15 urban community-based clinics in Baltimore, MD. Physicians who provided primary care to adult patients were eligible for enrollment January 2002–January 2003. Recruitment letters from the principal investigator and clinic medical directors offered physicians CME credit and individualized feedback on their communication styles.
Patients were eligible if their primary care physician was enrolled and if they were at least 18 years of age, English-speaking, and had at least one prior ICD-9 claim for hypertension in the past year. Eligibility was initially determined through claims data and chart reviews. Patients were recruited through mailed invitations followed by telephone calls if a refusal postcard from a particular patient was not received within 2 weeks. This strategy was supplemented with onsite recruitment at participating clinics. Patients were enrolled September 2003-August 2005. The median time between the physician burnout assessment and the patient encounter was 15.1 months (range 5.6–30).
Eligible physicians and patients who participated in a baseline audiotaped medical encounter comprised the sample for this analysis. Of 117 physicians assessed for eligibility, 111 responded to the invitation, with 58 refusing. Of the 53 who agreed to participate, 51 completed baseline questionnaires, and 1 was found to be ineligible. Of the 50 consenting randomized physician participants, 6 physicians in the minimal intervention group left their practice sites. In the intensive intervention group, three physicians withdrew (one for illness), and one did not participate in a baseline audiotaped encounter.
Thus, 40 physicians and 235 patients comprise the sample for this analysis. Compared with non-participants, our participants did not differ significantly by race (p = 0.384) or gender (p = 0.247).
Data Collection
An institutional review board of the Johns Hopkins Medical Institutions reviewed and approved the study procedures. All participating patients and physicians gave informed consent. Participating clinics received an incentive of $200 per physician, and patients were paid $25 per interview.
Questionnaires
During enrollment physicians completed baseline questionnaires, including demographic information and self-reported attitudes towards their work and patient care. The primary predictor variable – physician burnout – was measured using a six-item scale previously derived from the Maslach Burnout Inventory (MBI) to capture “job-related feelings of stress, lack of enthusiasm, and a reduced sense of accomplishment.”14 Burnout is conceptualized as a lasting state,24,25 which is indicated by the stability of the MBI over time.14,26 Our items captured the domains of emotional exhaustion and reduced sense of personal accomplishment,14 and respondents rated each item on a five-point Likert scale, from strongly disagree (1) to neutral (3) to strongly agree (5). Possible total scores range from 6 to 30, with higher scores representing greater burnout. The scale demonstrated high internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha of 0.85).
At the conclusion of their visits with patients, physicians completed brief questionnaires indicating the degree to which they knew the patient, their attitudes toward the patient in general, and their attitudes regarding that visit.
Interviewers administered baseline questionnaires to patients to obtain information about demographic characteristics and health status, including the Medical Outcomes Study Short Form-12 (SF-12)27 and self-reported co-morbidities.
Audiotaped Encounters and Analysis
The main outcome variables were rapport-building communication behaviors by physicians and patients during a single medical visit, corresponding to patient enrollment in the PPP Study.
At each enrollment visit, a research assistant set up and turned on an audio recorder in the examination room, leaving the room during the encounter. Physicians and patients could turn off the recorder at any time during the visit.
We analyzed audiotapes with the Roter Interaction Analysis System (RIAS), a widely used coding system for assessment of patient-physician communication.28–31 The system has established reliability and predictive validity.2,10,31,32 Two trained raters – blinded to the study hypotheses – assigned each complete thought expressed by patients or physicians to 1 of 37 categories of communication and 8 global affective domains. Interrater reliability on a subset of interviews (n = 23) averaged 85% (range 63% - 96%, calculated by Pearson’s correlations coefficients) for verbal communication codes with mean count >2.0 and 87–100% agreement for global affect ratings.
For this study, the main outcome variables consisted of four types of rapport-building in the patient-physician encounter, as follows33:
-
Positive rapport-building: The physician or patient offers statements characterized as laughter/tells jokes, direct approval, general compliment, or agreement/understanding.
-
Negative rapport-building: The physician or patient offers statements characterized as criticism or disagreement. This includes direct criticism towards or disagreement with the other person (doctor to patient or patient to doctor) or general criticism about people outside of the interaction (e.g., patient complaining about somebody at home). These statements build rapport by allowing honest expressions of differences in opinion or concerns about outside situations.
-
Emotional rapport-building: The physician or patient offers statements characterized as empathy, legitimation, concern/worry, reassurance/optimism, partnership, or self-disclosure.
-
Social rapport-building: The physician or patient offers personal remarks or engages in chit-chat about matters such as the weather or recent events.
Examples of statements related to each type of rapport-building are presented in Table 1. As demonstrated in these examples, rapport-building statements – even negative rapport-building through direct or general disagreement – are utterances that reflect engagement by patients or physicians in the encounter.31,33 The validity of rapport-building as a measure of patient-provider communication has been demonstrated in its associations with standardized patients’ ratings of communication quality,31 patients’ experience of care as patient-centered,34 and patient satisfaction.35
To investigate any differences between the content and emotional tone of the encounters, we also analyzed global affect ratings. In the RIAS system, these ratings are not associated with the content of the statements, but rather reflect the affect conveyed by the speakers through their vocal intonation and delivery.33 Using Likert scales (with 1 indicating low or none and 6 indicating high), coders rated (separately) the patients’ and physicians’ affect as conveyed by their verbal tone on nine dimensions: anger/irritation, emotional distress, dominance/assertiveness, interest/attentiveness, hurried/rushed, friendliness/warmth, responsiveness/engagement, sympathetic/empathetic, and respectfulness.33 We calculated composite variables to measure physician positive affect (the sum of ratings of interest, friendliness, responsiveness, sympathy, respectfulness, and the reverse coding of hurried/rushed) and patient positive affect (the sum of interest, friendliness, responsiveness, sympathy, and respectfulness).11,30
Patient-centeredness in the encounter was calculated by summing codes promoting the socioemotional, psychosocial, and biomedical agenda of the patient and dividing this by the sum of codes related to the biomedical agenda of the physician. A value greater than 1 denotes a more patient-centered encounter and a value less than 1 denotes a more physician-centered, biomedically oriented encounter.36–38
To calculate a verbal dominance ratio, we divided the total amount of physician statements by the total amount of patient statements. In this calculation, we did not include utterances such as “mm-hmm” or “uh-huh” (“back channel codes”) in which the speaker indicates attentiveness to the other person.33 Scores greater than 1 indicate more physician statements, and scores less than 1 indicate more patient statements.
During their in-person interviews, patients indicated their agreement (strongly disagree, agree, neither agree nor disagree, agree, or strongly agree) with the following statements:
-
Overall, I was satisfied with this visit.
-
I have confidence in this doctor’s knowledge and skills.
-
I trust this doctor to look out for my best interests.
Statistical Analysis
Burnout scores were divided into three categories – low, medium, and high – based on the distribution of the scale scores within the study sample. This decision is consistent with the scoring of the full Maslach Burnout Inventory (“low, average, and high”), as Maslach conceptualized burnout “as a continuous variable…not viewed as a dichotomous variable, which is either present or absent.”26 For three physicians with one to two missing responses to items (1.7% of burnout items), we imputed values using the average of the physician’s other responses to the burnout items.
We performed regression analyses using generalized estimating equations (GEE) to assess the presence and strength of relationships between physician burnout as the predictor and our primary outcomes of rapport-building by physicians and patients. Communication behaviors are likely to be similar in different encounters by the same physician. GEE adjusts the standard errors to account for within-physician correlations. For our models, we assumed an exchangeable correlation structure to allow for valid and robust estimates.39 Because the rapport-building outcomes were positively skewed counts with variances that exceeded their means, we analyzed the data using negative binomial distributions.40
First, we examined bivariate associations among several potential confounding variables and physician burnout. We chose a priori variables with empiric or strong theoretical relationships with patient-physician communication and physician well-being (see Fig. 1).41 These variables included: physician gender,2,7–9,42,43 race,10 age,2,44 practice site,15,45 years of practice, history of communication skills training, and whether the physician graduated from US or international medical school (IMG);46,47 patient gender, race,9 age,2 educational attainment, income,2 disease burden,2,7 SF-12 scores,2,7 and health insurance status; and the relationship variables of gender concordance (male-male, female-female, non-concordant),7,8 race concordance (yes or no),10 and physicians’ ratings of how well they knew patients on their post-visit questionnaires.
In our multivariate model, we retained variables with significance of p ≤ 0.10 in bivariate analyses (patient health insurance status and provider gender) and one variable (physician IMG status) that had a significant interaction with burnout. We also controlled for visit length. Physicians’ ratings of how well they knew each patient had no statistical relationship to burnout or IMG status and were not included in the final model. Patient and physician intervention status were not included in the multivariate model, as these had no significant relationships with either physician burnout or rapport-building behaviors during the baseline encounter.
We conducted bivariate and multivariate analyses of the relationship between the independent variable physician burnout and the secondary outcomes. We used GEE, with a continuous outcome distribution for patient-centeredness, positive affect, verbal dominance, and visit length, and a binomial distribution for patient ratings dichotomized as “strongly agree” vs. all other. The adjusted analyses again controlled for patient health insurance status, visit length, physician gender, physician IMG status, and interaction between IMG status and physician burnout.
We report outcomes with a significance level of p ≤ 0.001, to account for multiple comparisons. All analyses were performed using STATA Intercooled version 10.0 (Stata Statistical Software: Release 10.0, Stata Corporation, College Station, TX, 2007)
Results
Recruitment and Sample Characteristics
Among the 40 physicians, most were internists, 53% were women, and 30% were African-American. On average, physicians had practiced 11.2 years (see Table 2).
Among the 235 patients, two-thirds were women, and 60% were African-American. Although two-thirds reported incomes less than $35,000. Ninety percent of patients had health care insurance, 29% through Medicaid, 39% through Medicare, and 53% through private or work insurance (categories not mutually exclusive) (see Table 3).
The median number of patient encounters per physician was five. The median visit length was 14.7 min, and 39% of dyads were female gender concordant. Across the 235 encounters, the median number of coded verbal statements was 346 (interquartile range 243–484, total range 61–1,214).
Relationship Between Physician Burnout and Physician, Patient, and Relationship Characteristics
The mean burnout scale score was 14.0, with a range of 6 to 22. Fifteen physicians were in the high burnout category (score ≥17), 11 were in the medium burnout category (score ≥13 but <17), and 14 were in the low burnout category (score <13). There were no significant differences in burnout between the intervention and control group physicians.
Women physicians had higher burnout scores than male physicians (2.84, 95% CI -0.11 – 5.79, p = 0.06), as did US medical graduates compared with IMG physicians (3.02, 95% CI -0.56 – 6.60, p = 0.10), but these did not reach statistical significance. Other physician characteristics were not associated with burnout (data not shown).
In terms of patient-level characteristics, higher burnout scores were associated with insured patients (p = 0.02). Other patient or relationship characteristics were not associated with burnout (data not shown).
Association of Physician Burnout with Rapport-Building Behaviors by Physicians and Patients
No differences were evident in the rapport-building behaviors of physicians relative to their burnout scores (Table 4). However, patient communication during visits was related to physician burnout. Compared with patients of low-burnout physicians, patients of higher burnout physicians used nearly twice as many negative rapport-building statements (medium burnout incident risk ratio (IRR) 1.85, 95% CI 1.30 – 2.61, p = 0.001, and high burnout 2.06, 95% CI 1.48 – 2.86, p < 0.001) (see Table 4). Patients of high-burnout physicians were also 52% more likely to use reassurance/optimism statements (95% CI 1.40 – 1.66, p < 0.001), a type of emotional rapport-building behavior.
Associations Between Burnout and Other Communication Outcomes
Table 5 depicts additional bivariate analyses between physician burnout and the secondary communication outcomes. Individual global affect ratings did not differ significantly for physicians or patients across physician burnout categories (all p > 0.001, results not shown). The composite physician positive affect and patient positive affect scores did not differ significantly by physician burnout. Physician burnout was also not associated with significant differences in patient-centeredness, verbal dominance, or visit length.
Physician burnout was not associated with differences in the patients’ satisfaction with those particular visits, confidence in their physicians’ knowledge and skills, or trust in the physicians to look out for their best interests (Table 5).
DISCUSSION
Contrary to our hypothesis, physician burnout was not associated with differences in their own communication behaviors during medical visits. However, physician burnout was related to patient communication. Patients demonstrated more rapport-building of certain types in interactions with high burnout physicians.
Rapport-building exchanges serve an important function within the patient-physician encounter. Even disagreements with the physician or criticism directed toward others (the most frequent elements included in the negative rapport composite) may be seen as reflecting openness in the medical dialogue and solidarity with the physician. Higher patient engagement and participation in the medical encounter, including both positive and negative exchanges, are associated with patient’s experience of care as patient-centered34 and patient satisfaction.35
By other measures, higher burnout was not associated with lower quality physician communication. We found no evidence of poorer affective tone, less patient-centeredness, verbal dominance, shorter visits, or lower patient ratings. Our findings differ from previous studies, which have suggested that high burnout physicians have lower confidence in their communication skills, lower capacities for empathy, and suboptimal self-reported communication behaviors with patients.16,19,20
Several explanations may account for the difference between our findings and others. First, physicians with burnout may have unreasonably high expectations for themselves and may judge their performance more severely than other physicians or their patients. A prior study suggests that physicians significantly underestimate their patients’ positive attitudes towards them.48 Because of their reduced sense of personal accomplishment, physicians with burnout may be more likely to perceive suboptimal interactions with patients. As a result, physicians with burnout may actually perform as well as or better than their counterparts in their observed encounters, despite rating themselves as worse on self-reported questionnaires.19 A concerning implication of this explanation is the risk that excellent physicians could enter a vicious cycle where burnout leads to faulty perceptions of suboptimal performance, which then predisposes to worsening burnout. In a longitudinal study of physicians-in-training, West et al. found that physicians with self-perceived medical errors had increased odds of subsequent burnout and depression.49
Alternatively, physicians reporting burnout may be more sensitive to high patient expectations that they feel they cannot meet. Although our physicians completed the burnout scale prior to the specific patient encounter analyzed, these findings may suggest that high-burnout physicians have patient panels characterized by high patient engagement. Satisfaction with the patient-doctor relationship is associated with physicians’ global satisfaction,50 but physicians with high burnout may not perceive patient rapport-building as satisfying or successful. The higher-burnout physicians in our sample may tend to perceive patient rapport-building statements as placing demands on them. An et al. found that providers who perceive they have a higher proportion of “difficult” relationships are more likely to have symptoms of burnout.51 The “social exchange model of burnout” suggests that patients bring both demands and resources that may affect physician well-being and that physicians who perceive a lack of reciprocity in the patient-doctor relationship may be susceptible to burnout.52 Thus, although we measured burnout before assessing communication behaviors, the relationship could extend in the other direction, with patient-provider relationships affecting physician well-being.
Our findings may also relate to the balance between empathy and boundary-setting in the patient-provider encounter. Huggard termed this phenomenon “compassion fatigue,” in which physicians who engage empathically with their patients experience secondary traumatic stress and develop burnout.53 Gender effects may play a role in this as well. Shanafelt et al. found that women residents scored higher in empathy despite having lower well-being compared with men.42 This may suggest that – even early in their training – physicians are learning to put their patients’ well-being ahead of their own.54
Finally, patients may perceive unmeasured nonverbal cues from physicians with burnout that elicit an empathic response to these physicians. Patients’ overtures of rapport-building – such as reassurance/optimism statements – could represent patients’ efforts to demonstrate empathy or support for their physicians. We did not find any significant differences in the emotional tone of physicians relative to burnout; however, these cues may have been communicated through facial expressions or body language — a channel not rated by the coders.
The limitations of this study should be considered. First, we did not administer the complete MBI or include items from all three domains of burnout, and so our scale and categories may not represent clinically meaningful differences in the level of burnout among physicians. However, our questions demonstrated high internal consistency. Second, our sample included only a subset of patients from each physician’s panel, and we may have obtained different results with a larger or a randomly selected sample. Third, our sample of physicians and patients may not be representative of primary care encounters in other settings, particularly outside of urban, minority community clinics. Compared with a 2001 statewide random sample of Maryland physicians, our study physicians were younger, more likely to be women, and more likely to be an ethnic minority.55 Fourth, our communication analyses were limited to data from audiotaped encounters, and we were unable to assess the role of other cues – such as body language – in the interactions. Fifth, with only one visit recorded for each patient, our study cannot assess the directionality or causality in the relationship between burnout and communication outcomes. Finally, during the interval between the measurement of physician burnout and the measurement of communication behaviors in the patient encounters, burnout may have changed for some physicians. The MBI has demonstrated stability for up to 1 year, capturing the enduring state of burnout in diverse populations – including nurses.14,26,56–59 However, if burnout did change over time, we may have underestimated or overestimated the associations between burnout and communication behaviors. For example, we may have found a stronger relationship with patient rapport-building or ratings had we measured burnout closer to the time of the encounters.
More research is needed to understand the potential strength and mechanisms of a relationship between physician well-being and patient-physician communication. Our study focused on only one aspect of physician well-being: burnout. This analysis did not address other aspects of physician well-being – such as career satisfaction, coping strategies, or acute stressors – which could mediate or modify the relationship between burnout and communication.
In conclusion, our study found that higher physician burnout may be associated with more patient rapport-building behaviors and patients’ experiences of confidence and trust in their physicians. Future studies should investigate the complex links among clinician well-being, quality of medical visit communication, and patient outcomes.
References
Levinson W, Roter DL, Mullooly JP, Dull VT, Frankel RM. Physician-patient communication. The relationship with malpractice claims among primary care physicians and surgeons. JAMA. 1997 Feb 19;277(7):553–559.
Roter DL, Stewart M, Putnam SM, Lipkin M Jr, Stiles W, Inui TS. Communication patterns of primary care physicians. JAMA. 1997 Jan 22–29;277(4):350–356.
Hall JA, Roter DL, Katz NR. Meta-analysis of correlates of provider behavior in medical encounters. Med Care. 1988 Jul;26(7):657–675.
Stewart MA. Effective physician-patient communication and health outcomes: A review. CMAJ. 1995 May 1;152(9):1423–1433.
Kaplan SH, Greenfield S, Ware JE Jr. Assessing the effects of physician-patient interactions on the outcomes of chronic disease. Med Care. 1989 Mar;27(3 Suppl):S110–S127.
Clever SL, Ford DE, Rubenstein LV, et al. Primary care patients’ involvement in decision-making is associated with improvement in depression. Med Care. 2006 May;44(5):398–405.
Hall JA, Horgan TG, Stein TS, Roter DL. Liking in the physician–patient relationship. Patient Educ Couns. 2002 Sep;48(1):69–77.
Hall JA, Roter DL. Do patients talk differently to male and female physicians? A meta-analytic review. Patient Educ Couns. 2002 Dec;48(3):217–224.
Cooper-Patrick L, Gallo JJ, Gonzales JJ, et al. Race, gender, and partnership in the patient-physician relationship. JAMA. 1999 Aug 11;282(6):583–589.
Cooper LA, Roter DL, Johnson RL, Ford DE, Steinwachs DM, Powe NR. Patient-centered communication, ratings of care, and concordance of patient and physician race. Ann Intern Med. 2003 Dec 2;139(11):907–915.
Beach MC, Roter DL, Wang NY, Duggan PS, Cooper LA. Are physicians’ attitudes of respect accurately perceived by patients and associated with more positive communication behaviors? Patient Educ Couns. 2006 Sep;62(3):347–354.
Fernandez A, Schillinger D, Grumbach K, et al. Physician language ability and cultural competence. an exploratory study of communication with Spanish-speaking patients. J Gen Intern Med. 2004 Feb;19(2):167–174.
Cvengros JA, Christensen AJ, Hillis SL, Rosenthal GE. Patient and physician attitudes in the health care context: Attitudinal symmetry predicts patient satisfaction and adherence. Ann Behav Med. 2007 May-Jun;33(3):262–268.
Maslach C, Jackson SE, Leiter MP. Maslach Burnout Inventory: Third edition. In: Zalaquett CP, Wood RJ, eds. Evaluating stress: A book of resources. Scarecrow Education; 1997:191–218.
Linzer M, Visser MR, Oort FJ, et al. Predicting and preventing physician burnout: Results from the United States and the Netherlands. Am J Med. 2001 Aug;111(2):170–175.
Travado L, Grassi L, Gil F, Ventura C, Martins C, Southern European Psycho-Oncology Study Group. Physician-patient communication among southern European cancer physicians: The influence of psychosocial orientation and burnout. Psychooncology. 2005 Aug;14(8):661–670.
Halbesleben JR. Patient reciprocity and physician burnout: What do patients bring to the patient-physician relationship? Health Serv Manag Res. 2006 Nov;19(4):215–222.
Williams ES, Manwell LB, Konrad TR, Linzer M. The relationship of organizational culture, stress, satisfaction, and burnout with physician-reported error and suboptimal patient care: Results from the MEMO study. Health Care Manage Rev. 2007 Jul-Sep;32(3):203–212.
Shanafelt TD, Bradley KA, Wipf JE, Back AL. Burnout and self-reported patient care in an internal medicine residency program. Ann Intern Med. 2002 Mar 5;136(5):358–367.
Thomas MR, Dyrbye LN, Huntington JL, et al. How do distress and well-being relate to medical student empathy? A multicenter study. J Gen Intern Med. 2007 Feb;22(2):177–183.
Argentero P, Dell’Olivo B, Ferretti MS. Staff burnout and patient satisfaction with the quality of dialysis care. Am J Kidney Dis. 2008 Jan;51(1):80–92.
Halbesleben JR, Rathert C. Linking physician burnout and patient outcomes: Exploring the dyadic relationship between physicians and patients. Health Care Manage Rev. 2008 Jan-Mar;33(1):29–39.
Patient-Physician Partnership to Improve High Blood Pressure Adherence Study. ClinicalTrials.gov identifier: NCT00123045, Last updated January 18, 2008. ClinicalTrials.gov, U.S. National Institutes of Health. Available at http://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT00123045?term = partnership+and+adherence&rank=3. Accessed June 5, 2008.
McManus IC, Keeling A, Paice E. Stress, burnout and doctors’ attitudes to work are determined by personality and learning style: A twelve year longitudinal study of UK medical graduates. BMC Med. 2004 Aug 18;2:29.
Piedmont RL. A longitudinal analysis of burnout in the health care setting: The role of personal dispositions. J Pers Assess. 1993 Dec;61(3):457–473.
Maslach C, Jackson SE, Leiter MP. Maslach Burnout Inventory manual, 3rd edition. Mountain View, CA: CPP, Inc., 1996.
Ware J Jr, Kosinski M, Keller SD. A 12-item short-form health survey: Construction of scales and preliminary tests of reliability and validity. Med Care. 1996 Mar;34(3):220–233.
Roter DL, Stewart M, Putnam SM, Lipkin M Jr, Stiles W, Inui TS. Communication patterns of primary care physicians. JAMA. 1997 Jan 22–29;277(4):350–356.
Levinson W, Roter DL, Mullooly JP, Dull VT, Frankel RM. Physician-patient communication. The relationship with malpractice claims among primary care physicians and surgeons. JAMA. 1997 Feb 19;277(7):553–559.
Cooper LA, Roter DL, Johnson RL, Ford DE, Steinwachs DM, Powe NR. Patient-centered communication, ratings of care, and concordance of patient and physician race. Ann Intern Med. 2003 Dec 2;139(11):907–915.
Price EG, Windish DM, Magaziner J, Cooper LA. Assessing validity of standardized patient ratings of medical students’ communication behavior using the Roter Interaction Analysis System. Patient Educ Couns. 2008 Jan;70(1):3–9.
Roter D, Larson S. The Roter Interaction Analysis System (RIAS): Utility and flexibility for analysis of medical interactions. Patient Educ Couns. 2002 Apr;46(4):243–251.
Roter D. The Roter method of interaction process analysis2007 March;21:55.
Ishikawa H, Hashimoto H, Roter DL, Yamazaki Y, Takayama T, Yano E. Patient contribution to the medical dialogue and perceived patient-centeredness. An observational study in Japanese geriatric consultations. J Gen Intern Med. 2005 Oct;20(10):906–910.
Bertakis KD, Roter D, Putnam SM. The relationship of physician medical interview style to patient satisfaction. J Fam Pract. 1991 Feb;32(2):175–181.
Ford S, Fallowfield L, Lewis S. Doctor-patient interactions in oncology. Soc Sci Med. 1996 Jun;42(11):1511–1519.
Mead N, Bower P. Measuring patient-centredness: A comparison of three observation-based instruments. Patient Educ Couns. 2000 Jan;39(1):71–80.
Paasche-Orlow M, Roter D. The communication patterns of internal medicine and family practice physicians. J Am Board Fam Pract. 2003 Nov-Dec;16(6):485–493.
Zeger SL, Liang KY. Longitudinal data analysis for discrete and continuous outcomes. Biometrics 1986 Mar;42(1):121–130.
Byers AL, Allore H, Gill TM, Peduzzi PN. Application of negative binomial modeling for discrete outcomes: A case study in aging research. J Clin Epidemiol. 2003 Jun;56(6):559–564.
Roter D, Hall JA. Doctors talking with patients. Patients talking with doctors: Improving communication in medical visits. 2nd ed. Westport, Conn.: Praeger; 2006.
Shanafelt TD, West C, Zhao X, et al. Relationship between increased personal well-being and enhanced empathy among internal medicine residents. J Gen Intern Med. 2005 Jul;20(7):559–564.
Stewart DE, Ahmad F, Cheung AM, Bergman B, Dell DL. Women physicians and stress. J Womens Health Gend Based Med. 2000 Mar;9(2):185–190.
Goehring C, Bouvier Gallacchi M, Kunzi B, Bovier P. Psychosocial and professional characteristics of burnout in Swiss primary care practitioners: A cross-sectional survey. Swiss Med Wkly. 2005 Feb 19;135(7–8):101–108.
Piette JD, Schillinger D, Potter MB, Heisler M. Dimensions of patient-provider communication and diabetes self-care in an ethnically diverse population. J Gen Intern Med. 2003 Aug;18(8):624–633.
Morris AL, Phillips RL, Fryer GE Jr, Green LA, Mullan F. International medical graduates in family medicine in the United States of America: An exploration of professional characteristics and attitudes. Hum Resour Health. 2006 Jul 18;4:17.
Laird SL, Beach MC, Cooper LA. International medical graduates report more difficulties delivering patient-centered care than U. S. medical graduates. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2006;21(4):76.
Hall JA, Stein TS, Roter DL, Rieser N. Inaccuracies in physicians’ perceptions of their patients. Med Care. 1999 Nov;37(11):1164–1168.
West CP, Huschka MM, Novotny PJ, et al. Association of perceived medical errors with resident distress and empathy: A prospective longitudinal study. JAMA. 2006 Sep 6;296(9):1071–1078.
Suchman AL, Roter D, Green M, Lipkin M Jr. Physician satisfaction with primary care office visits. Collaborative study group of the American Academy on Physician and Patient. Med Care. 1993 Dec;31(12):1083–1092.
An PG, Rabatin JS, Brown RL, Manwell LB, Linzer M, Schwartz MD. Effects of caring for difficult patients: Data from the MEMO (Minimizing Error, Maximizing Outcome) study. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2007 Apr;22:125.
Halbesleben JR. Patient reciprocity and physician burnout: What do patients bring to the patient-physician relationship? Health Serv Manage Res. 2006 Nov;19(4):215–222.
Huggard P. Compassion fatigue: How much can I give? Med Educ. 2003 Feb;37(2):163–164.
Ratanawongsa N, Wright SM, Levine RB. Association between higher mental well-being and residents’ capacity for empathy. J Gen Intern Med. 2006 Apr;21(4):401,2; author reply 402.
Center for Health Program Development and Management. The Maryland study on physician experience with managed care. 2001.
Deary IJ, Watson R, Hogston R. A longitudinal cohort study of burnout and attrition in nursing students. J Adv Nurs. 2003 Jul;43(1):71–81.
Jackson SE, Schwab RL, Schuler RS. Toward an understanding of the burnout phenomenon. J Appl Psychol. 1986 11;71(4):630–640.
Leiter MP, Durup MJ. Work, home, and in-between: A longitudinal study of spillover. J Appl Behav Sci. 1996 03;32(1):29–47.
Richardsen AM, Martinussen M. Factorial validity and consistency of the MBI-GS across occupational groups in Norway. Int J Stress Manag. 2005 08;12(3):289–297.
Acknowledgements
These data have been presented in abstract form at the 2007 International Conference for Communication in Healthcare Research Forum in Charleston, SC. This study was conducted with funding from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute grants nos. R01HL069403 and K24HL083113 (Dr. Cooper). The funding agency did not have a role in the design, conduct, or reporting of the study. Dr. Lisa Cooper had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
Conflict of interest: None disclosed.
Author contributions:
Study concept and design: Ratanawongsa, Roter, Larson, Beach, Laird, Carson, Cooper
Acquisition of data: Roter, Larson, Carson, Cooper
Analysis and interpretation of data: Ratanawongsa, Roter, Larson, Beach, Laird, Cooper
Drafting of the manuscript: Ratanawongsa
Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Ratanawongsa, Roter, Larson, Beach, Laird, Carson, Cooper
Statistical expertise: Carson
Obtained funding: Cooper
Administrative, technical, or material support: Carson, Cooper
Study supervision: Cooper
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Ratanawongsa, N., Roter, D., Beach, M.C. et al. Physician Burnout and Patient-Physician Communication During Primary Care Encounters. J GEN INTERN MED 23, 1581–1588 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11606-008-0702-1
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11606-008-0702-1