Advertisement

Cognitive Neurodynamics

, Volume 12, Issue 2, pp 201–209 | Cite as

Dancers and fastball sports athletes have different spatial visual attention styles

  • Ummuhan Isoglu-Alkac
  • M. Numan Ermutlu
  • Gökçer Eskikurt
  • İlker Yücesir
  • Sernaz Demirel Temel
  • Tan Temel
Research Article

Abstract

Physical exercise and the training effects of repeated practice of skills over an extended period of time may have additive effects on brain networks and functions. Various motor skills and attentional styles can be developed by athletes engaged in different sports. In this study, the effects of fast ball sports and dance training on attention were investigated by event related potentials (ERP). ERP were recorded in auditory and visual tasks in professional dancer, professional fast ball sports athlete (FBSA) and healthy control volunteer groups consisting of twelve subjects each. In the auditory task both dancer and FBSA groups have faster N200 (N2) and P300 (P3) latencies than the controls. In the visual task FBSA have faster latencies of P3 than the dancers and controls. They also have higher P100 (P1) amplitudes to non-target stimuli than the dancers and controls. On the other hand, dancers have faster latencies of P1 and higher N100 (N1) amplitude to non-target stimuli and they also have higher P3 amplitudes than the FBSA and controls. Overall exercise has positive effects on cognitive processing speed as reflected on the faster auditory N2 and P3 latencies. However, FBSA and dancers differed on attentional styles in the visual task. Dancers displayed predominantly endogenous/top down features reflected by increased N1 and P3 amplitudes, decreased P1 amplitude and shorter P1 latency. On the other hand, FBSA showed predominantly exogenous/bottom up processes revealed by increased P1 amplitude. The controls were in between the two groups.

Keywords

Event related potentials Fast ball sports Dance Spatial visual attention 

Notes

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the athletes, the dancers and their coaches for their cooperation and participation in this study. The results of the present study do not constitute endorsement by the American College of Sports Medicine.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Basar E (2013) Brain oscillations and neuropsychiatry. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 15:291–300PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  2. Bashore TR (1989) Age, physical fitness, and mental processing speed. Ann Rev Gerontol Geriatr 9:120–144Google Scholar
  3. Belardinelli R, Lacalaprice F, Ventrella C, Volpe L, Faccenda E (2008) Waltz dancing in patients with chronic heart failure new form of exercise training. Circ Heart Fail 1:107–114CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Bianco V, Di Russo F, Perri RL, Berchicci M (2017) Different proactive and reactive action control in fencers’ and boxers’ brain. Neuroscience 343:260–268CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Bläsing B, Tenenbaum G, Schack T (2009) The cognitive structure of movements in classical dance. Psychol Sport Exerc 10:350–360CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Brown WS, Marsh JT, LaRue A (1982) Event-related potentials in psychiatry: differentiating depression and dementia in the elderly. Bull Los Angel Neurol Soc 47:91–107Google Scholar
  7. Brümmer V, Schneider S, Abel T, Vogt T, Strüder HK (2011) Brain cortical activity is influenced by exercise mode and intensity. Med Sci Sports Exerc 43(10):1863–1872CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Bundesen C (1990) A theory of visual attention. Psychol Rev 97(4):523–547CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Busse L, Katzner S, Tillmann C, Treue S (2008) Effects of attention on perceptual direction tuning curves in the human visual system. J Vis 8(9):1–13CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Carrasco M (2011) Visual attention: the past 25 years. Vis Res 51(13):1484–1525CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  11. Chica AB, Bartolomeo P, Lupiánez J (2013) Two cognitive and neural systems for endogenous and exogenous spatial attention. Behav Brain Res 237:107–123CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Corbetta M, Shulman GL (2002) Control of goal-directed and stimulus driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:201–215CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Corbetta M, Patel G, Shulman GL (2008) The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron 58(3):306–324CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  14. Coull JT, Nobre AC (1998) Where and when to pay attention: the neural systems for directing attention to spatial locations and to time intervals as revealed by both PET and fMRI. J Neurosci 18(18):7426–7435PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Desimone R, Duncan J (1995) Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Ann Rev Neurosci 18:193–222CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Dustman RE, Emmerson RY, Ruhling RO, Shearer DE, Steinhaus LA, Johnson SC, Bonekat HW, Shigeoka JW (1990) Age and fitness effects on EEG, ERPs, visual sensitivity, and cognition. Neurobiol Aging 11:193–200CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Emmerson RY, Dustman RE, Shearer DE, Turner C (1989) P3 latency and symbol digit performance correlations in aging. Exp Aging Res 15:15l–159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Ermutlu N, Yucesir İ, Eskikurt G, Temel T, İşoğlu-Alkaç U (2015) Brain electrical activities of dancers and fast ball sports athletes are different. Cogn Neurodyn 9(2):257–263CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Fontani G, Maffei D, Cameli S, Polidori F (1999) Reactivity and event-related potentials during attentional tests in athletes. Eur J Appl Physiol 80:308–317CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Fox MD, Corbetta M, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Raichle ME (2006) Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral attention systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:1046–1051CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Hein E, Rolke B, Ulrich R (2006) Visual attention and temporal discrimination: differential effects of automatic and voluntary cueing. Vis Cogn 13(1):29–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hillman CH, Erickson K, Kramer AF (2008) Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 9:58–65CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Iwadate M, Mori A, Ashizuka T, Takayose M, Ozawa T (2005) Long-term physical exercise and somatosensory event-related potentials. Exp Brain Res 160:528–532CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Kramer AF, Erickson KI (2007) Effects of physical activity on cognition, well-being, and brain: human interventions. Alzheimer Dement 3:45–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Kutas M, McCarthy G, Donchin E (1977) Augmenting mental chronometry: the P300 as a measure of stimulus evaluation time. Science 197:792–795CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Lavoie ME, Dupuis F, Johnston KM, Leclerc S, Lassonde M (2004) Visual P300 effects beyond symptoms in concussed college athletes. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 26(1):55–73CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Linden DEJ, Prvulovic D, Formisano E, Vollinger M, Zanella FE, Goebel R, Dierks T (1999) The functional neuroanatomy of target detection: an fRMI study of visual and auditory oddball tasks. CerebralCortex 9:815–823Google Scholar
  28. Ling S, Carrasco M (2006) Sustained and transient covert attention enhance the signal via different contrast response functions. Vis Res 46(8–9):1210–1220CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Liu T, Stevens ST, Carrasco M (2007) Comparing the time course and efficacy of spatial and feature-based attention. Vis Res 47(1):108–113CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Marquez CMS, Vanaudenaerde B, Troosters BT, Wenderoth N (2015) High-intensity interval training evokes larger serum BDNF levels compared with intense continuous exercise. J Appl Physiol 119:1363–1373CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Minvielle-Moncla J, Audiffren M, Macar F, Vallet C (2008) Overproduction timing errors in expert dancers. J Mot Behav 40:291–300CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Nakata H, Yoshie M, Miura A, Kudo K (2010) Characteristics of the athletes’ brain: evidence from neurophysiology and neuroimaging. Brain Res Rev 62:197–211CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Nakayama K, Mackeben M (1989) Sustained and transient components of focal visual attention. Vis Res 29(11):1631–1647CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Picton TW, Hillyard SA (1988) Endogenous event-related potentials. In: Picton TW (ed) Human event-related potentials. Elsevier, New York, pp 361–426Google Scholar
  35. Polich J, Martin S (1992) P300, cognitive capability and personality: a correlational study of university undergraduates. Personal Individ Differ 13:533–543CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Polich J, Ehlers CL, Otis S, Mandell A, Bloom FE (1986) P300 latency reflects the degree of cognitive decline in dementing illness. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 63:138–144CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Remington RW, Johnston JC, Yantis S (1992) Involuntary attentional capture by abrupt onsets. Percept Psychophys 51(3):279–290CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Reynolds JH, Heeger DJ (2009) The normalization model of attention. Neuron 61:168–185CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  39. Ritter W, Vaughan HG (1969) Averaged evoked responses in vigilance and discrimination: a reassessment. Science 164:326–328CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Rossi B, Zani A, Taddei F, Pesce C (1992) Chronometric aspects of information processing in high level fencers as compared to non-athletes: an ERPs and RT study. J Hum Mov Stud 23:17–28Google Scholar
  41. Runigo CL, Benguigui N, Bardy BG (2010) Visuo-motor delay, information movement coupling, and expertise in ball sports. J Sports Sci 28:327–337CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Schubert M, Johannes S, Koch M (1998) Differential effects of two motor tasks on ERPs in an auditory classification task: evidence of shared cognitive resources. Neurosci Res 30:125–134CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Stoppel CM, Boehler CN, Strumpf H, Krebs RM, Heinze HJ, Hopf JM, Schoenfeld MA (2013) Distinct representations of attentional control during voluntary and stimulus-driven shifts across objects and locations. Cereb Cortex 23:1351–1361CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Taddei F, Bultrini A, Spinelli D, Di Russo F (2013) Neural correlates of attentional and executive processing in middle-age fencers. Med Sci Sports Exerc 44(6):1057–1066CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Thompson T, Steffert T, Ros T, Leach J, Gruzelier J (2008) EEG applications for sport and performance. Methods 45:279–288CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Wickens C, Kramer A, Vanasse L, Donchin E (1983) The performance of concurrent tasks: a psychophysiological analysis of the reciprocity of information processing resources. Science 221:1080–1082CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Yantis S, Jonides J (1984) Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: evidence from visual search. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 10:601–621CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V., part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Ummuhan Isoglu-Alkac
    • 1
  • M. Numan Ermutlu
    • 2
  • Gökçer Eskikurt
    • 3
  • İlker Yücesir
    • 4
  • Sernaz Demirel Temel
    • 5
  • Tan Temel
    • 5
  1. 1.Department of Physiology, Istanbul Faculty of MedicineIstanbul UniversityIstanbulTurkey
  2. 2.Department of Physiology, Faculty of MedicineIstanbul Bilim UniversityIstanbulTurkey
  3. 3.Department of Physiology, Faculty of MedicineIstinye UniversityIstanbulTurkey
  4. 4.School of Physical Education and SportsIstanbul UniversityIstanbulTurkey
  5. 5.Department of Music and Performing Arts, The Art and Design FacultyYTUIstanbulTurkey

Personalised recommendations