The primary aim of the current paper was to examine whether community members prioritised indicators of wellbeing, mental health, or both mental health and wellbeing to define flourishing. Further we compared whether those indicators chosen to reflect flourishing differed from those indicators prioritised as reflecting QoL; just how much do flourishing and QoL overlap? Finally, we considered the extent socio-demographic and health characteristics were associated with definitions of quality of life and flourishing.
There are a number of important findings to elucidate. First, it was highlighted that participants defined ‘flourishing’ and ‘a good quality of life’ in terms of a combination of both mental health and wellbeing. Based on the responses of our 646 survey participants, we note that most (13) of the indicators were selected as reflective of QoL and flourishing by more than 20% of the sample. This suggests that there is no consistency between community members in how either flourishing or QoL are defined. Further, it is important to highlight that greater proportions of items selected were derived from the wellbeing indicators, (e.g. ‘having people around who really care about me’, ‘feeling that what you do in your life is valuable and worthwhile’, ‘being able to bounce back when things go wrong’). Although some mental health indicators, namely ‘having a sense of worth’, ‘enjoying a good quality sleep’, ‘being free of excessive worries/anxieties that are difficult to manage’, and ‘experiencing pleasure in most activities’, were also selected by at least 20% of the respondents. Conversely, only 4 indicators, specifically mental health indicators with a somatic element, including ‘ability to concentrate on task at hand’, ‘not experiencing fatigue’, ‘being free of muscle tension’, and ‘not feeling irritable’, were selected by less than 10% of the sample. Indeed, the latter 2 indicators were selected by only 2.5% and 1.5% of the sample respectively. This suggests that wellbeing indicators appear to hold slightly more weight for individuals in describing flourishing and QoL, at least for those respondents in this study. However, when asked to rank those items selected, the differences in the extent to which mental health or wellbeing indicators were ranked was less substantive.
Second, we can conclude that the chosen indicators of both QoL and Flourishing are highly comparable. Overall, there were few differences between the two surveys in the proportions of respondents identifying each indicator as indicative of QoL or Flourishing. Any notable differences were for low prevalence indicators. For example, more respondents in the Flourishing survey (4.2%) were likely to select ‘being free of muscle tension’ in comparison with the QoL survey (1.1%); however, the overall rate of selection was very low < 5%. There were notable differences in terms of the wellbeing indicators ‘having people around who really care about me’ and ‘having a sense of accomplishment’ with 43.9% and 26.8% of respondents in the Flourishing survey (Survey 2) selecting these items in comparison with 54.0% and 17.8% of respondents in the QoL survey (Survey 1). There also was a notable difference in terms of the mental health indicator ‘being free of excessive worries and anxieties that are difficult to manage’ with 29.2% of respondents in the QoL survey selecting this item in comparison with 20.6% of respondents in the Flourishing survey. Otherwise, there were no other substantive differences between surveys in the proportions of participants selecting the mental health and wellbeing indicators. This suggests that, at a measurement level, QoL and Flourishing may be conceptually highly similar to members of the community.
Third, and perhaps of most importance, is that no one indicator was endorsed by more than 50% of the sample. Although 54.0% of the QoL sample endorsed ‘having people around who really care about me’ (vs. 43.9% in the Flourishing sample), these findings suggest that there is no particular feature of wellbeing or mental health which unanimously captures Flourishing or QoL. Instead, respondents endorsed a large number of the available indicators. These similarities were also noted in the rank order of items between the QoL and Flourishing samples. Perhaps of particular interest for wellbeing researchers was that strongly endorsed wellbeing indicators reflected social, psychological and subjective wellbeing suggesting all three dimensions are perceived as equally important. That there was no dominant feature for defining Flourishing is similar to results on happiness, where happiness has been defined in terms of family (29%), relationships (26.9%), a sense of harmony (25.4%), with no single factor reported by a majority of respondents (Delle Fave et al., 2011).
Finally, we examined whether the indicators of quality of life and flourishing were related to socio-demographic participant characteristics. Several associations were reported. First, individuals with higher wellbeing were less likely to endorse indicators associated with anxiety (e.g. ‘being free of muscle tension’) and depression (e.g. ‘being free of depressed mood’). This suggests that the immediate salience of indicators may be important in community conceptualisations of what it means to flourish or have a good quality of life. For example, for those with poorer wellbeing, the importance of being free of depression may be vital, whereas for those already experiencing positive wellbeing they instead emphasise indicators that may elevate positive wellbeing further such as being ‘interested in learning new thing’ and ‘being optimistic about the future’. Being male was associated with higher likelihood of reporting ‘having a sense of accomplishment’, but lower likelihood of reporting ‘having people around who really care about me’ and ‘being able to bounce back when things go wrong’. This is consistent with research examining gender differences in value priorities and definitions such that females prioritise social connection and personal relationships where material success and accomplishment are reported by males (Dyke & Murphy, 2006).
Other notable socio-demographic characteristics were partner status and level of contact. Surprisingly, being partnered was associated with lower likelihood of reporting ‘having people around who really care about me’. It is possible that this may because individuals that are partnered take the support benefits for granted and don’t see it as something that drives their wellbeing. Being partnered was also associated with higher likelihood of reporting ‘being free of excessive worries and anxieties that are difficult to manage’ which is line with existing literature which highlights that those in good quality relationships report better wellbeing and mental health (Umberson & Montez, 2010; Williams, 2003). Level of contact with mental health was not associated with endorsement of mental health or wellbeing indicators.
Together these findings may inform clinical practice and health policy. On one hand it is useful to know that community conceptualisations of flourishing and QoL are largely the same regardless of the terminology they were presented. However, the finding that only approximately 50% of the sample endorse one item, at most – most items ranged from 17 to 45%, highlights the diverse ways in which the community values and understands flourishing or quality of life to mean. Whilst some may prioritise social connections, others value having a sense of worth, resilience, positive experiences or mastery. This implies that measurement of community and individuals’ wellbeing must consider the breadth and multi-dimensional (and to an extent the hierarchical structure) nature of wellbeing. Otherwise, purported differences between individuals or groups of individuals, may otherwise be simply an artifact of underlying measurement issues.
The endorsement of a combination of mental health and wellbeing indicators adds complexity to the flourishing literature. Common definitions of flourishing are defined in terms of wellbeing or positive mental health only (Diener et al., 2010; Hone et al., 2014; Huppert & So, 2013; Keyes, 2002). Whilst wellbeing indicators were generally more frequently endorsed in our study, a review of the mental health indicators still shows that 5 of the 9 mental health indicators were endorsed as indicators of flourishing by 17.1% to 44.3% of respondents. This is a substantial number who define flourishing in terms of mental health or specifically the lack of psychopathology. Further, a comparison of the rank order of items endorsed reveals that respondents ranked 2 mental health indicators (‘sense of worth’; ‘free of depressed mood’) in the top 5, and 2 more (‘free of excessive worries and anxiety’; ‘not experiencing fatigue’) in the top 10 of ranked indicators. We therefore believe there is an argument to reconcile current theoretical frameworks of Flourishing (and consequently Languishing) which are informed by wellbeing, or positive mental health, only (Diener et al., 2010; Hone et al., 2014; Huppert & So, 2013; Keyes, 2002) following the results of our community surveys. That is, researchers define flourishing in terms of the presence of wellbeing, and languishing in terms of the absence wellbeing. There may be significant limitations for current flourishing frameworks where existing flourishing definitions are based on wellbeing (note, the term positive mental health is often used synonymously), as the results from our community survey suggest a number of mental health indicators, in addition to wellbeing, reflect community members’ perceptions of this construct.
Consequently, we propose a conceptual model, The Total Psychological Health Framework of Flourishing and Languishing (See Fig. 4) which incorporates existing theoretical frameworks of flourishing and languishing, and the dual continua of mental health and wellbeing (Diener et al., 2010; Huppert & So, 2013; Keyes, 2002, 2005, 2007). First, individuals are placed on scales of mental health (high vs. low levels of symptomology/distress) and wellbeing (low vs. high levels of wellbeing) independently. Generally we would expect at least small to moderate negative correlations between where individuals may sit on these dimensions. Second, in contrast to current definitions of flourishing and languishing which are identified on the figure, and reflected by level of wellbeing only, we propose defining flourishing in terms of the concordance of both wellbeing and mental health. Flourishing therefore reflects the presence of both high mental health (i.e. low psychological distress/symptomology) and high wellbeing. Conversely, languishing is defined as the presence of both low mental health (i.e. high psychological distress/symptomology) and low wellbeing. Our proposed definitions of flourishing and languishing are described by the dotted lines that encircle both ends of the mental health and wellbeing axes. This proposition is supported by the results of our community participants who prioritised both dimensions of wellbeing and mental health as reflecting flourishing. Conversely, we hypothesize that Languishing, can be defined as the absence of wellbeing and presence of mental illness symptoms. Our current study did not examine languishing specifically, and clearly this is a hypothesis that needs to be tested.
We note that in some instances defining flourishing as the concordance of wellbeing and psychopathology (high well-being + low psychopathology) has been proposed (Keyes, 2004), but the extent to which this model is applied is inconsistent. And a corresponding definition of languishing (in terms of the concordance of wellbeing and psychopathology) has not been made, as far as we are aware. We therefore consider it important that a clear and declarative model outlines unambiguiously how flourishing and languishing are defined in terms of both dimensions of wellbeing and mental health.
We recognise that much of this discussion is around the concordance participants may experience (i.e. high mental health and wellbeing; low mental health and wellbeing), but other combinations of wellbeing and mental health may be possible (see Keyes (2002)) where individuals may report moderate on one dimension, and high, or low, on another dimension. However, we believe it is also important to caution over-interpreting some of these findings, specifically that individuals can experience high wellbeing whilst reporting mental illness. Indeed, we note that Keyes’ (2002) mental illness was based on a binary indicator drawn from 12-month CIDI diagnosis – whereas wellbeing was defined on current state. Whilst we strongly advocate that those who experience periods of mental illness can still experience wellbeing throughout their lives, and hence capacity to flourish, we believe it is highly unlikely that individuals with current high wellbeing can experience concurrent psychological distress that would be sufficient to inhibit individuals’ daily functional capacity. Indeed, Criteria B for MDD, for example, would most likely specifically preclude this. At this stage, we would emphasise here a need to distinguish between ever and current diagnosis, and specifically treated vs. non-treated current ill-health. Simply, if someone is symptomatic (i.e. currently experiencing symptoms of mental illness or is currently unwell), it is unlikely that they would report high levels of flourishing across multiple wellbeing dimensions, including mood. But that person, when managing their illness (e.g. in receipt of treatment), has potential to flourish. It is highly feasible for the individual in remission or not experiencing an episode, to be afforded the capacity to flourish. Indeed, we would highlight that several indicators of major depressive disorder (e.g. presence of depressed mood, lack of positive mood, feelings of worthlessness) directly contrast with wellbeing indicators (e.g. lack of negative affect, presence of positive mood, a sense of worth) and so the likelihood of individuals being able to ‘flourish’ within existing frameworks AND experiencing significant psychological disturbance is problematic. We therefore argue here, in line with our community participants’ responses, that having positive mental health or wellbeing alone does not reflect a flourishing state. Rather we posit that experiencing both wellbeing and mental health reflects a flourishing status; the converse reflects languishing.
The main argument we make here is based on the results from our community members who appear to rank mental health as an important indicator of flourishing. There is clearly scope for further research to consider how and in what ways complexity in wellbeing and mental health co-occur. However, while individuals with psychiatric illness may experience flourishing in periods of their lives, it is questionable to suggest that individuals can be flourishing when reporting a psychiatric disorder as identified by current flourishing definitions (Diener et al., 2010; Huppert & So, 2013; Keyes, 2002, 2005, 2007). Importantly, it is unclear the extent to which current flourishing models identify individuals at-risk. We believe it is important for both clinical practice and public health policy to clearly distinguish between wellbeing and mental health dimensions, since wellbeing is itself a risk for future mental health outcomes (Burns et al., 2011, 2022; Fava et al., 2001, 2011; Lamers et al., 2015; Ruini & Fava, 2009; Weich et al., 2011; Wood & Joseph, 2010), and consequently that definitions of personal flourishing should be based in terms of lives being lived well, with a sense of emotional, psychological and social wellbeing, AND the absence of CURRENT psychopathology/mental illness which inhibit daily functioning. We believe such a clear distinction has important implications for emphasising differences in promoting positive wellbeing and addressing mental illness in the community. We propose that our model proposed here may be an avenue for structuring further research in the area which examines the nexus between wellbeing and mental health.
Limitations and Future Directions
While the current study provides important insight into the way in which community members identify wellbeing and mental health indicators as important in reflecting Flourishing and a Quality of Life, several limitations should be acknowledged. First, is it important to note that even when considering the 10 most endorsed indicators, they were only endorsed between approximately 25% and 50% of the sample. This highlights the difficulties in defining and establishing clear differentiation between QoL and Flourishing constructs. Second, we note that many of the socio-demographic predictors found to be related to perceptions of QoL and Flourishing are of marginal substantive difference. Therefore, the extent of any statistical significance identified needs to be considered with caution in light of the sample size. Generally, there was consistency in the way individuals emphasised mental health and wellbeing indicators for both QoL and Flourishing. However, we note that there were some differences in socio-demographic and health characteristics between the surveys which assessed Quality of Life (Survey 1) and Flourishing (Survey 2) separately and we recognise that these between-person factors may account for the study findings.
There are several areas for future consideration. First, despite the large sample size, the study needs to be replicated with other community samples. Also, we make no assertion that the findings from our community sample would reflect specific populations, particularly those at greater risk for poor mental health (e.g. university students, older adults in residential care, those with particular chronic illnesses). Relatedly, owing to the broad age range of our sample (age 18–84 years), we have not examined age differences in the results presented here. Study designs which utilise multiple narrow age-cohorts (e.g. 20–25; 40–45; 60–65; 80–85 years) would be best for eliciting whether there are age-related differences in the indicator elicited as reflective of flourishing and quality of life. Relatedly, studies need to consider longitudinal stability in participant responses in order to discriminate between-person and within-person differences, where within-person differences may reflect ageing related (or other external contextual factors) changes in indicator preferences. Finally, we have focused on eliciting from respondents which indicators they prioritise as the most important for describing flourishing and quality of life. We need to replicate these findings in terms of how community persons define languishing. As the current findings implicate both the absence of mental illness symptoms and the presence of positive wellbeing indicators in defining flourishing/quality of life, it will be important to determine whether the presence of mental illness symptoms and absence of wellbeing indicators are similarly related to perceptions of languishing or whether it is primarily the presence of mental illness symptoms or absence of wellbeing indicators that are prioritised in defining languishing. We recognise that in order to control for the valence of the DSM and ESS indicators, DSM mental health symptoms were rephrased to reflect a positive state or absence of symptom. It would be important to extend this method and examine whether managing mental illness symptoms; in that respects, some individuals may define their flourishing as being able to manage mental illness symptoms as well as prioritising wellbeing. Relatedly latent class/mixture analysis of wellbeing and mental health symptoms could identify relatively homogenous groups of individuals who have particular combinations of wellbeing and mental health symptoms; for example, are there groups of individuals who have active symptoms of mental illness and have positive well-being?