Skip to main content
Log in

Modulation of Physiological Responses as Indices of Attentional Bias in Dysphoria

  • Original Article
  • Published:
International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The experimental results on attentional bias in depression have been contradictory. Research on physiological correlates that validate attentional bias have been scarce. The aim of the present study was to evaluate physiological responses associated with attentional bias in dysphoria. Heart rate and the galvanic skin response were recorded in 30 dysphoric individuals and 30 nondysphoric individuals while they performed an emotional Stroop task. Dysphoric individuals exhibited attentional bias and heart rate deceleration in response to unpleasant words. Nondysphoric individuals exhibited attentional bias toward pleasant words, accompanied by a higher skin conductance response. These results suggest that heart rate deceleration in response to unpleasant words correlated with attentional bias in dysphoric individuals.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Pleasant words: kiss, orgasm, love, caress. Neutral words: pencil, paper, board, cell. Unpleasant words: war, bomb, abuse, torture.

References

  • Barsky, A. J., Goodson, J. D., Lane, R. S., & Cleary, P. D. (1988). The amplification of somatic symptoms. Psychosomatic Medicine, 50(5), 510–519.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bassett, D. (2016). A literature review of heart rate variability in depressive and bipolar disorders. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 50(6), 511–519. doi:10.1177/0004867415622689.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the BDI-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blanchette, I., & Richards, A. (2013). Is emotional Stroop interference linked to affective responses? Evidence from skin conductance and facial electromyography. Emotion, 13(1), 129–138. doi:10.1037/a0029520.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36(2), 129–148 Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/amp/index.aspx.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M. M. (2009). Natural selective attention: orienting and emotion. Psychophysiology, 46(1), 1–11. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00702.x.

  • Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1999). Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW): Instruction manual and affective ratings. Gainesville, FL: Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Cuthbert, B., & Lang, P. J. (2001). Emotion and motivation I: Defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion, 1(3), 276–298. doi:10.1037//1528-3542.1.3.276.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brunoni, A. R., Kemp, A. H., Dantas, E. M., Goulart, A. C., Nunes, M. A., Boggio, P. S., et al. (2013). Heart rate variability is a trait marker of major depressive disorder: evidence from the sertraline vs. electric current therapy to treat depression clinical study. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 16(9), 1937–1949. doi:10.1017/S1461145713000497.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, B. A., Wood, G., & McBride, T. (1997). Origins of orienting and defensive responses: An evolutionary perspective. In P. J. Lang, R. F. Simons, & M. T. Balaban (Eds.), Attention and orienting: Sensory and motivational processes (pp. 41–67). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheng, P., Preston, S. D., Jonides, J., Mohr, A. H., Thummala, K., Casement, M., et al. (2015). Evidence against mood-congruent attentional bias in major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Research, 230(2), 496–505. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2015.09.043.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D. A., Beck, A. T., & Alford, B. (1999). Scientific foundations of cognitive theory and therapy of depression. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc..

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., & McClelland, J. L. (1990). On the control of automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 332–361 Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/bul/index.aspx.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Critchley, H. D., Elliott, R., Mathias, C. J., & Dolan, R. J. (2000). Neural activity relating to generation and representation of galvanic skin conductance responses: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Neuroscience, 20(8), 3033–3040.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dalgleish, T., & Watts, F. N. (1990). Biases of attention and memory in disorders of anxiety and depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 10, 589–604. doi:10.1016/0272-7358(90)900098-U.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epp, A. M., Dobson, K. S., Dozois, D. J., & Frewen, P. A. (2012). A systematic meta-analysis of the Stroop task in depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 32(4), 316–328. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2012.02.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Erdfelder, E., Faul, F., & Buchner, A. (1996). GPOWER: a general power analysis program. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28, 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fridrici, C., Driessen, M., Wingenfeld, K., Kremer, G., Kissler, J., & Beblo, T. (2014). Investigating biases of attention and memory for alcohol-related and negative words in alcohol-dependents with and without major depression after day-clinic treatment. Psychiatry Research, 218(3), 311–318. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2014.03.041.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gantiva, C., & Camacho, K. (2016). Características de la respuesta emocional generada por las palabras: Un estudio experimental desde la emoción y la motivación. Psychologia: Avances de la Disciplina, 10(2), 55–62.

  • Gantiva, C., Delgado, R., & Romo-González, T. (2015). Emotional reactions to alcohol-related words: Differences between low-and high-risk drinkers. Addictive Behaviors, 50, 60–63. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.06.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guerra, P. M., Sánchez-Adam, A., Miccoli, L., Polich, J., & Vila, J. (2016). Heart rate and P300: Integrating peripheral and central indices of cognitive processing. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 100, 1–11. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.12.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hauschildt, M., Wittekind, C., Moritz, S., Kellner, M., & Jelinek, L. (2013). Attentional bias for affective visual stimuli in posttraumatic stress disorder and the role of depression. Psychiatry Research, 207(1), 73–79. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2012.11.024.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jin, A. B., Steding, L. H., & Webb, A. K. (2015). Reduced emotional and cardiovascular reactivity to emotionally evocative stimuli in major depressive disorder. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 97(1), 66–74. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.04.014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kendall, P. C., Hollon, S. C., Beck, A. T., Hammen, C. L., & Ingram, R. E. (1987). Issues and recommendations regarding the use of the Beck depression inventory. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11, 289–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klorman, R., Weissbert, R. P., & Wiessenfeld, A. R. (1977). Individual differences in fear and autonomic reactions to affective stimulation. Psychophysiology, 14, 45–51.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koster, E. H., De Raedt, R., Goeleven, E., Franck, E., & Crombez, G. (2005). Mood-congruent attentional bias in dysphoria: maintained attention to and impaired disengagement from negative information. Emotion, 5(4), 446–455. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.5.4.446.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koster, E. H., De Raedt, R., Leyman, L., & De Lissnyder, E. (2010). Mood-congruent attention and memory bias in dysphoria: Exploring the coherence among information-processing biases. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(3), 219–225. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2009.11.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kreibig, S. D. (2010). Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: A review. Biological Psychology, 84(3), 394–421. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J. (1979). A bio-informational theory of emotional imagery. Psychophysiology, 16, 495–512. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1979.tb01511.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). Motivated attention: Affect, activation, and action. In P. J. Lang, R. F. Simons, & M. T. Balaban (Eds.), Attention and orienting: Sensory and motivational processes (pp. 97–135). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109(2), 163–203. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maltzman, I., & Boyd, G. (1984). Stimulus significance and bilateral SCRs to potentially phobic pictures. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 41–46.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mathersul, D., McDonald, S., & Rushby, J. A. (2013). Automatic facial responses to briefly presented emotional stimuli in autism spectrum disorder. Biological Psychology, 94(2), 397–407. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.08.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1994). Cognitive approaches to emotion and emotional disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 45, 25–50. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.45.020194.000325.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mineka, S., Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1998). Comorbidity of anxiety and unipolar mood disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 377–412. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.377.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2005). Attentional bias in generalized anxiety disorder versus depressive disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29(1), 29–45. doi:10.1007/s10608-005-1646-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Redondo, J., Fraga, I., Padrón, I., & Comesaña, M. (2007). The Spanish adaptation of ANEW (Affective Norms for English Words). Behavior Research Methods, 39(3), 600–605.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Richard-Devantoy, S., Ding, Y., Turecki, G., & Jollant, F. (2016). Attentional bias toward suicide-relevant information in suicide attempters: a cross-sectional study and a meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 196, 101–108. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2016.02.046.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sanz, J., Navarro, M. E., & Vázquez, C. (2003). Adaptación española del Inventario para la Depresión de Beck-II (BDI-II): Propiedades psicométricas en estudiantes universitarios. Análisis y Modificación de Conducta, 29(124), 239–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sass, S. M., Heller, W., Fisher, J. E., Silton, R. L., Stewart, J. L., Crocker, L. D., et al. (2014). Electrophysiological evidence of the time course of attentional bias in non-patients reporting symptoms of depression with and without co-occurring anxiety. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 301. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00301.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, D., Regenbogen, C., Kellermann, T., Finkelmeyer, A., Kohn, N., Derntl, B., et al. (2012). Empathic behavioral and physiological responses to dynamic stimuli in depression. Psychiatry Research, 200(2), 294–305. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2012.03.054.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sokolov, E. N. (1963). Perception and the conditioned reflex. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Storrie, M. C., Doerr, H. O., & Johnson, M. H. (1981). Skin conductance characteristics of depressed subjects before and after therapeutic intervention. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 169(3), 176–179.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643–662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toone, B. K., Cooke, E., & Lader, M. H. (1981). Electrodermal activity in the affective disorders and schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 11(3), 497–508.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ward, N. G., Doerr, H. O., & Storrie, M. C. (1983). Skin conductance: a potentially sensitive test for depression. Psychiatry Research, 10(4), 295–302.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wenzler, S., Hagen, M., Tarvainen, M. P., Hilke, M., Ghirmai, N., Huthmacher, A. C., et al. (2017). Intensified emotion perception in depression: differences in physiological arousal and subjective perceptions. Psychiatry Research, 253, 303–310. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2017.03.040.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Carlos Gantiva.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Funding

Funding for this study was provided by the University of San Buenaventura (Ref. PSI 011-015).University of San Buenaventura, had no role in the study design, collection, analysis or interpretation of the data, writing the manuscript, or the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gantiva, C., Araujo, A., Aragão, N. et al. Modulation of Physiological Responses as Indices of Attentional Bias in Dysphoria. Int J Ment Health Addiction 16, 328–338 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-017-9774-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-017-9774-7

Keywords

Navigation