Advertisement

Executive Function Deficits in Psychiatric Outpatients in Australia

  • T. P. S. OeiEmail author
  • S. Shaw
  • K . L. Healy
Article
  • 144 Downloads

Abstract

This study compared deficits in executive functioning in psychiatric outpatients with anxiety and depression to neurologically impaired patients and a community sample using a cross-sectional design. Anxious, depressed and neurologically impaired patients were compared for executive dysfunction using the revised Dysexecutive Questionnaire. A large sample of 663 community individuals, 39 mood disordered, 121 anxiety disordered and 120 neurologically impaired outpatients participated. Anxious and depressed patients exhibited significantly greater executive dysfunction than neurological patients and healthy controls. Anxiety patients reported the greatest executive dysfunction than all other groups including greater total problems and specific deficits in inhibiting responses and in acting with volition. Problems with inhibition reflect difficulties of anxiety patients in preventing automatic responses. Problems with volition reported by anxiety patients were attributed to comorbid depression, consistent with anxiety patients reporting the highest levels of depressive as well as anxious symptomology. Depressed patients reported greater problems with volition than community members, reflecting problems with planning and motivation. Neurologically impaired individuals reported a comparable level of executive dysfunction to the general community. This was attributed mainly to the poor ability of neurologically impaired individuals to report their own deficits. We discuss the relevance of executive functioning to anxiety and depression, and the implications of using the DEX-R with clinical samples.

Keywords

DEX-R Executive function Executive dysfunction Dysexecutive syndrome Depression Anxiety disorders 

Notes

Acknowledgments

Dr. Oei is now an Emeritus Professor of UQ and also a visiting Professor of James Cook University, Singapore and Nanjing University, Pr China. We would like to thank the participants, in particular the outpatients who participated in this study.

References

  1. Airaksinen, E., Larsson, M., & Forsell, Y. (2005). Neuropsychological functions in anxiety disorders in population-based samples: evidence of episodic memory dysfunction. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 39(2), 207–214.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV-TR. Washington: American Psychiatric Publications.Google Scholar
  3. Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
  4. Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 1–24.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Beck, A. T., Epstein, N., Brown, G., & Steer, R. A. (1988). An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56(6), 893–897.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Briggs, S. R., & Cheek, J. M. (1986). The role of factor analysis in the development and evaluation of personality scales. Journal of Personality, 54, 106–148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Brown, T. A., & Barlow, D. H. (2009). A proposal for a dimensional classification system based on the shared features of the DSM-IV anxiety and mood disorders: implications for assessment and treatment. Psychological Assessment, 21(3), 256–271.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  8. Burgess, P. W., Alderman, N., Wilson, B. A., Evans, J. J., & Emslie, H. (1996). The dysexecutive questionnaire. Behavioural assessment of the dysexecutive syndrome. Bury St. Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Company.Google Scholar
  9. Burgess, P. W., Alderman, N., Evans, J., Emslie, H., & Wilson, B. A. (1998). The ecological validity of tests of executive function. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 4(06), 547–558.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Chan, R. C. (2001). Dysexecutive symptoms among a non-clinical sample: a study with the use of the dysexecutive questionnaire. British Journal of Psychology, 92, 551–565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Christopher, G., & MacDonald, J. (2005). The impact of clinical depression on working memory. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 10(5), 379–399.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (Eds.) (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  13. de Zubicaray, G. (2006). Neuroimaging and clinical neuropsychological practice (chapter 4). In P. J. Snyder, P. D. Nussbaum, & D. L. Robins (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology: A pocket handbook for assessment (pp. 56–74). Washington: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Ghika-Schmid, F., & Bogousslavsky, J. (1997). Affective disorders following stroke. European Neurology, 38(2), 75–81.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Henry, G. M., Weingartner, H., & Murphy, D. L. (1973). Influence of affective states and psychoactive drugs on verbal learning and memory. American Journal of Psychiatry, 130(9), 966–971.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Hertel, P. T., & Hardin, T. S. (1990). Remembering with and without awareness in a depressed mood: evidence of deficits in initiative. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119(1), 45–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Ladouceur, R., Blais, F., Freeston, M. H., & Dugas, M. J. (1998). Problem solving and problem orientation in generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 12(2), 139–152.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Lautenbacher, S., Spernal, J., & Krieg, J. C. (2002). Divided and selective attention in panic disorder. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 252(5), 210–213.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D., & Loring, D. (Eds.) (2004). Neuropsychological assessment (4th Ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  20. Lord, F. M. (1969). Statistical adjustments when comparing preexisting groups. Psychological Bulletin, 68(5), 336–337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Lucas, J. A., & Addeo, R. (2006). Traumatic brain injury and postconcussion syndrome. In P. J. Snyder, P. D. Nussbaum, & D. L. Robins (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology: A pocket handbook for assessment (pp. 351–380). Washington: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Ludewig, S., Ludewig, K., Geyer, M. A., Hell, D., & Vollenweider, F. X. (2002). Prepulse inhibition deficits in patients with panic disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 15(2), 55–60.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. MacQueen, G. M., Tipper, S. P., Young, L. T., Joffe, R. T., & Levitt, A. J. (2000). Impaired distractor inhibition on a selective attention task in unmedicated, depressed subjects. Psychological Medicine, 30(03), 557–564.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. McGeorge, T. (2006). Neuropsychology of PTSD: biological, cognitive, and clinical perspectives. International Review of Psychiatry, 18(2), 193–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Mialet, J. P., Pope, H. G., & Yurgelun-Todd, D. (1996). Impaired attention in depressive states: a non-specific deficit? Psychological Medicine, 26(05), 1009–1020.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Mooney, B., Walmsley, C., & McFarland, K. (2006). Factor analysis of the self-report dysexecutive (DEX-S) questionnaire. Applied Neuropsychology, 13(1), 12–18.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Nitschke, J. B., & Mackiewicz, K. L. (2005). Prefrontal and anterior cingulate contributions to volition in depression. International Review of Neurobiology, 67, 73–94.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Ponsford, J. L., Olver, J. H., & Curran, C. (1995). A profile of outcome: 2 years after traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 9(1), 1–10.Google Scholar
  29. Rao, V., & Lyketsos, C. G. (2000). The benefits and risks of ECT for patients with primary dementia who also suffer from depression. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 15(8), 729–735.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Rozenthal, M., Laks, J., & Engelhardt, E. (2004). Aspectos neuropsicológicos da depressão. Revista de Psiquiatria, 26(2), 204–212.Google Scholar
  31. Saver, J. L., & Damasio, A. R. (1991). Preserved access and processing of social knowledge in a patient with acquired sociopathy due to ventromedial frontal damage. Neuropsychologia, 29(12), 1241–1249.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Shaw, S., Oei, T. P., & Sawang, S. (2015). Psychometric validation of the dysexecutive questionnaire (DEX). Psychological Assessment, 27(1), 138–147.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Stordal, E., Morken, G., Mykletun, A., Neckelmann, D., & Dahl, A. A. (2008). Monthly variation in prevalence rates of comorbid depression and anxiety in the general population at 63–65° north: the HUNT study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 106(3), 273–278.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Wilson, B. A., Alderman, N., Burgess, P. W., Emslie, H., & Evans, J. J. (1996). Behavioural assessment of the dysexecutive syndrome. Bury St Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Company.Google Scholar
  35. Zung, W. W. (1965). A self-rating depression scale. Archives of General Psychiatry, 12(1), 63–70.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.The University of QueenslandBrisbaneAustralia
  2. 2.James Cook UniversitySingaporeSingapore
  3. 3.Nanjing UniversityNanjingPeoples Republic of China

Personalised recommendations