Ackerman, R. (2014). The diminishing criterion model for metacognitive regulation of time investment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(3), 1349–1368.
Article
Google Scholar
Ackerman, R., & Goldsmith, M. (2008). Control over grain size in memory reporting –with and without satisficing knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(5), 1224–1245.
Article
Google Scholar
Ackerman, R., & Goldsmith, M. (2011). Metacognitive regulation of text learning: on screen versus on paper. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 17(1), 18–32.
Google Scholar
Ackerman, R., & Lauterman, T. (2012). Taking reading comprehension exams on screen or on paper? A metacognitive analysis of learning texts under time pressure. Computers in Human Behavior, 28(5), 1816–1828.
Article
Google Scholar
Ackerman, R., & Zalmanov, H. (2012). The persistence of the fluency–confidence association in problem solving. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19(6), 1189–1192.
Article
Google Scholar
Alter, A. L., Oppenheimer, D. M., Epley, N., & Eyre, R. N. (2007). Overcoming intuition: metacognitive difficulty activates analytic reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136(4), 569–576.
Article
Google Scholar
Annand, D. (2008). Learning efficacy and cost-effectiveness of print versus e-book instructional material in an introductory financial accounting course. Journal of Interactive Online Learning, 7(2), 152–164.
Google Scholar
Antón, C., Camarero, C., & Rodríguez, J. (2013). Usefulness, enjoyment, and self-image congruence: the adoption of e-book readers. Psychology & Marketing, 30(4), 372–384.
Article
Google Scholar
Ball, R., & Hourcade, J. P. (2011). Rethinking reading for age from paper and computers. International Journal of Human-Computer Interaction, 27(11), 1066–1082.
Article
Google Scholar
Benedetto, S., Drai-Zerbib, V., Pedrotti, M., Tissier, G., & Baccino, T. (2013). E-readers and visual fatigue. PLoS One, 8(12), 1–7.
Article
Google Scholar
Benjamin, A. S., Bjork, R. A., & Schwartz, B. L. (1998). The mismeasure of memory: when retrieval fluency is misleading as a metamnemonic index. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 55–68.
Article
Google Scholar
Ben-Yehudah, G., & Eshet-Alkalai, Y. (2014). The influence of text annotation tools on print and digital reading comprehension. In Y. Eshet, A. Caspi, N. Geri, Y. Kalman, V. Silber-Varod, & Y. Yair (Eds.), Proceedings of the 9th Chais Conference for Innovation in Learning Technologies (pp. 28–35). Raanana, Israel: Open University Press.
Bjork, R. A. (1994). Memory and metamemory considerations in the training of human beings. In J. Metcalfe & A. Shimamura (Eds.), Metacognition: Knowing about knowing (pp. 185–205). Cambridge: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Bjork, R. A. (1999). Assessing our own competence: Heuristics and illusions. In D. Gopher & A. Koriat (Eds.), Attention and performance XVII: Cognitive regulation of performance: Interaction of theory and application (pp. 435–459). Cambridge: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Chiong, C., Ree, J., Takeuchi, L., & Erickson, I. (2012). Comparing parent–child co-reading on print, basic, and enhanced e-book platforms. The Joan Ganz Cooney Center. http://www.joanganzcooneycenter.org/publication/quickreport-print-books-vs-e-books/
Cokely, E. T., & Kelley, C. M. (2009). Cognitive abilities and superior decision making under risk: a protocol analysis and process model evaluation. Judgment and Decision Making, 4(1), 20–33.
Google Scholar
Daniel, D. B., & Woody, W. D. (2013). E-textbooks at what cost? Performance and use of electronic v. print texts. Computers & Education, 62, 18–23.
Article
Google Scholar
DeStefano, D., & LeFevre, J. (2007). Cognitive load in hypertext reading: a review. Computers in Human Behavior, 23, 1616–1641.
Article
Google Scholar
Diemand-Yauman, C., Oppenheimer, D. M., & Vaughan, E. B. (2011). Fortune favors the bold (and the italicized): effects of disfluency on educational outcomes. Cognition, 118(1), 111–115.
Article
Google Scholar
Dunlosky, J., & Thiede, K. W. (1998). What makes people study more? An evaluation of factors that affect self-paced study. Acta Psychologica, 98(1), 37–56.
Article
Google Scholar
Dunlosky, J., & Thiede, K. W. (2013). Four cornerstones of calibration research: why understanding students’ judgments can improve their achievement. Learning and Instruction, 24, 58–61.
Article
Google Scholar
Eden, S., & Eshet-Alkalai, Y. (2013). The effect of format on performance: editing text in print versus digital formats. British Journal of Educational Technology, 44(5), 846–856.
Article
Google Scholar
Evans, J. S. B. T., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition: advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3), 223–241.
Article
Google Scholar
Frederick, S. (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19, 25–42.
Article
Google Scholar
Greene, J. A., & Azevedo, R. (2007). A theoretical review of Winne and Hadwin’s model of self-regulated learning: new perspectives and directions. Review of Educational Research, 77(3), 334–372.
Article
Google Scholar
Gu, X., Wu, B., & Xu, X. (2015). Design, development, and learning in e-textbooks: what we learned and where we are going. Journal of Computers in Education, 2(1), 25–41.
Article
Google Scholar
Hillesund, T. (2010). Digital reading spaces: How expert readers handle books, the Web and electronic paper. First Monday (Online). Retrieved from http://firstmonday.org/htbin/cgiwrap/bin/ojs/index.php/fm/article/view/2762/2504
Hirshman, E., & Mulligan, N. (1991). Perceptual interference improves explicit memory but does not enhance data-driven processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17(3), 507–513.
Article
Google Scholar
Holzinger, A., Baernthaler, M., Pammer, W., Katz, H., Bjelic-Radisic, V., & Ziefle, M. (2011). Investigating paper vs. screen in real-life hospital workflows: performance contradicts perceived superiority of paper in the user experience. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 69(9), 563–570.
Article
Google Scholar
King, A. (1991). Effects of training in strategic questioning on children’s problem-solving performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83(3), 307–317.
Article
Google Scholar
Koriat, A., & Ackerman, R. (2010). Metacognition and mindreading: judgments of learning for self and other during self-paced study. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(1), 251–264.
Article
Google Scholar
Koriat, A., Ma’ayan, H., & Nussinson, R. (2006). The intricate relationships between monitoring and control in metacognition: lessons for the cause-and-effect relation between subjective experience and behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135(1), 36–69.
Article
Google Scholar
Kühl, T., & Eitel, A. (this issue). Effects of Disfluency on Cognitive and Metacognitive Processes and Outcomes. Metacognition and Learning.
Lauterman, T., & Ackerman, R. (2014). Overcoming screen inferiority in learning and calibration. Computers in Human Behavior, 35, 455–463.
Article
Google Scholar
Leeson, H. V. (2006). The mode effect: a literature review of human and technological issues in computerized testing. International Journal of Testing, 6(1), 1–24.
Article
Google Scholar
Li, L. Y., Chen, G. D., & Yang, S. J. (2013). Construction of cognitive maps to improve e-book reading and navigation. Computers & Education, 60(1), 32–39.
Article
Google Scholar
Li, F., Xie, R., Li, X., & Li, W. (2015). The influence of perceptual information on control processes involved in self-regulated learning: evidence from item selection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(4), 1007–1013.
Article
Google Scholar
Liu, Z. (2005). Reading behavior in the digital environment. Journal of Documentation, 61(6), 700–712.
Article
Google Scholar
Mangen, A., Walgermo, B. R., & Brønnick, K. (2013). Reading linear texts on paper versus computer screen: effects on reading comprehension. International Journal of Educational Research, 58, 61–68.
Article
Google Scholar
Margolin, S. J., Driscoll, C., Toland, M. J., & Kegler, J. L. (2013). E-readers, computer screens, or paper: does reading comprehension change across media platforms? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 27, 512–519.
Article
Google Scholar
Masson, M. E., & Rotello, C. M. (2009). Sources of bias in the Goodman–Kruskal gamma coefficient measure of association: implications for studies of metacognitive processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(2), 509–527.
Article
Google Scholar
Mayer, R. E., Heiser, J., & Lonn, S. (2001). Cognitive constraints on multimedia learning: when presenting more material results in less understanding. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(1), 187–198.
Article
Google Scholar
Meyer, A., Frederick, S., Burnham, T., Guevara Pinto, J. D., Boyer, T. W., Ball, L. J., Pennycook, G., Ackerman, R., Thompson, V., & Schuldt, J. P. (2015). Disfluent fonts don’t help people solve math problems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(2), e16–e30.
Article
Google Scholar
Mizrachi, D. (2015). Undergraduates’ academic reading format preferences and behaviors. The Journal of Academic Librarianship, 41(3), 301–311.
Article
Google Scholar
Morineau, T., Blanche, C., Tobin, L., & Guéguen, N. (2005). The emergence of the contextual role of the e-book in cognitive processes through an ecological and functional analysis. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 62(3), 329–348.
Article
Google Scholar
Murray, M. C., & Pérez, J. (2011). E-textbooks are coming: are we ready? Issues in Informing Science and Information Technology, 8, 49–60.
Google Scholar
Nelson, T. O. (1984). A comparison of current measures of the accuracy of feeling-of-knowing predictions. Psychological Bulletin, 95(1), 109–133.
Article
Google Scholar
Norman, E., & Furnes, B. (2016). The relationship between metacognitive experiences and learning: is there a difference between digital and non-digital study media? Computers in Human Behavior, 54, 301–309.
Article
Google Scholar
Oeberst, A., Haberstroh, S., & Gnambs, T. (2015). Not really the same: computerized and real lotteries in decision making research. Computers in Human Behavior, 44, 250–257.
Article
Google Scholar
Oppenheimer, D. M. (2006). Consequences of erudite vernacular utilized irrespective of necessity: problems with using long words needlessly. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20(2), 139–156.
Article
Google Scholar
Poole, A., & Ball, L. J. (2006). Eye tracking in HCI and usability research. Encyclopedia of Human Computer Interaction, 1, 211–219.
Article
Google Scholar
Prowse Turner, J. A., & Thompson, V. A. (2009). The role of training, alternative models, and logical necessity in determining confidence in syllogistic reasoning. Thinking & Reasoning, 15(1), 69–100.
Article
Google Scholar
Salmerón, L., & García, V. (2012). Children’s reading of printed text and hypertext with navigation overviews: the role of comprehension, sustained attention, and visuo-spatial abilities. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 47(1), 33–50.
Article
Google Scholar
Shynkaruk, J. M., & Thompson, V. A. (2006). Confidence and accuracy in deductive reasoning. Memory & Cognition, 34(3), 619–632.
Article
Google Scholar
Soderstrom, N. C., Clark, C. T., Halamish, V., & Bjork, E. L. (2015). Judgments of learning as memory modifiers. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(2), 553–558.
Article
Google Scholar
Song, H., & Schwarz, N. (2008). If it’s hard to read, it’s hard to do: processing fluency affects effort prediction and motivation. Psychological Science, 19(10), 986–988.
Article
Google Scholar
Sungkhasettee, V. W., Friedman, M. C., & Castel, A. D. (2011). Memory and metamemory for inverted words: illusions of competency and desirable difficulties. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(5), 973–978.
Article
Google Scholar
Tauber, S. K., & Rhodes, M. G. (2012). Measuring memory monitoring with judgements of retention (JORs). The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 1376–1396.
Article
Google Scholar
Thiede, K. W., Anderson, M., & Therriault, D. (2003). Accuracy of metacognitive monitoring affects learning of texts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(1), 66–73.
Article
Google Scholar
Thompson, V. A. (2009). Dual process theories: A metacognitive perspective. In J. Evans & K. Frankish (Eds.), In two minds: Dual processes and beyond (pp. 171–195). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Thompson, V. A., Turner, J. A. P., Pennycook, G., Ball, L. J., Brack, H., Ophir, Y., & Ackerman, R. (2013). The role of answer fluency and perceptual fluency as metacognitive cues for initiating analytic thinking. Cognition, 128(2), 237–251.
Article
Google Scholar
Winne, P. H. (2004). Students’ calibration of knowledge and learning processes: implications for designing powerful software learning environments. International Journal of Educational Research, 41(6), 466–488.
Article
Google Scholar
Woody, W. D., Daniel, D. B., & Baker, C. A. (2010). E-books or textbooks: students prefer textbooks. Computers & Education, 55(3), 945–948.
Article
Google Scholar
Yue, C. L., Castel, A. D., & Bjork, R. A. (2013). When disfluency is—and is not—a desirable difficulty: the influence of typeface clarity on metacognitive judgments and memory. Memory & Cognition, 41(2), 229–241.
Article
Google Scholar